The outbreak of the SARS-CoV-2 novel coronavirus (COVID-19) pandemic has led to a significant increase in mental health problems in many countries around the world (e.g., Gavin et al., 2020; Holmes et al., 2020; Torales et al., 2020), particularly anxiety (Luo et al., 2020; Malhotra et al., 2020; Mazza et al., 2020). The social distancing regulations and lockdowns that have been enforced in many countries as a means of containing the virus’s spread may be responsible for much of this anxiety (Brooks et al., 2020; Imran et al., 2020; Lei et al., 2020). Israel, like most countries around the world, has experienced devastating health, economic, and social effects of the pandemic. A large national survey conducted by Israel’s Central Bureau of Statistics (CBS) in May 2020 (CBS, 2020) found that 22.5% of surveyed adults (aged 21 and above) reported an increase in mental health difficulties, with anxiety and stress being the most prominent symptoms, experienced by 34.3% of the responders.

COVID-19 is also associated with increased risks for emotional and behavioral problems in children (Domínguez-Álvarez et al., 2020; Ghosh et al., 2020). Reports from around the world have documented an increase in children’s fears, inattention, irritability, regression, and clinginess (Jiao et al., 2020), as well as difficulties in concentration, restlessness, nervousness, and worries (Orgilés et al., 2020). It has also been noted that preschoolers are showing more vulnerability and behavioral problems compared to older children (Romero et al., 2020). Home confinement appears to have increased the risk of children developing behavior problems (Romero et al., 2020). Similarly, in the Israeli survey cited above (CBS, 2020), 25.8% of the responders said that their children’s emotional state had worsened since the COVID-19 outbreak. The global rise in children’s COVID-19-related behavioral and emotional difficulties has been attributed to the unique combination of the health crisis, the related economic recession, and disruption of families’ daily routines, home confinement, social isolation, and physical distancing from extended family, classmates, and teachers (Golberstein et al., 2020; Imran et al., 2020).

In light of these reports, the first goal of the current study was to assess differences in levels of children’s problematic behaviors and their mothers’ anxiety symptoms in the context of the COVID-19 outbreak vs. the pre-COVID era, using samples that had similar demographics. Furthermore, we were interested in examining to what degree the pandemic impacted children’s behavior problems directly, and to what degree the impact was indirect, mediated via their mothers’ anxiety and mentalizing abilities.

Maternal Anxiety, COVID-19, and Child’s Behavior Problems

As noted above, COVID-19 has been associated with an increase in anxiety, particularly among mothers. Maternal anxiety is considered a risk factor for children’s development, predicting later emotional, cognitive, and behavioral difficulties (Behrendt et al., 2019) in clinical and non-clinical samples (Gibler et al., 2018). Studies conducted by the current researchers in Israel prior to the pandemic showed that parents’ anxiety symptoms were linked with reports of their children’s externalizing and internalizing behaviors (Hanetz-Gamliel et al., 2018; Dollberg et al., 2021). Moreover, it is well established that in the presence of external threats, primary caregivers’ emotional reactions, particularly their anxiety levels, play a significant role in their offspring’s responses (Cobham et al., 2016; Masten & Narayan, 2012). Thus, not surprisingly, recent research found that parents’ depression and anxiety symptoms during the pandemic were associated with increases in children’s behavior problems and particularly internalizing behaviors (Saddik et al., 2020; Whittle et al., 2020).

The rise in maternal anxiety in the wake of the COVID-19 outbreak may stem from the medical, economic, and social threats associated with the outbreak as well as from the parental stress that has been linked with it. The virus and the measures taken to control its spread, (i.e., home confinement, school closures, and social distancing) have resulted in parents juggling between working from home and assisting their children with their remote homeschooling tasks, all of which has reportedly resulted in high parental stress and likely increased parents’ mental health difficulties (Coyne et al., 2020). Furthermore, parental stress has been reported to be the key mediating link to children’s behavioral and emotional problems during the pandemic, triggering parents’ anxiety and depressive symptoms, which negatively impacted parenting behaviors and parent–child interactions and children’s psychological and behavioral difficulties (Romero et al., 2020; Spinelli et al., 2020). It has been argued that COVID-19-stressed parents appear to be less able to understand their children’s needs and to respond to them sensitively (Spinelli et al., 2020), but the specific mechanism which accounts for this phenomenon has not been elucidated or tested.

Maternal Mentalization

Maternal mentalization is a trans-theoretical, multidimensional concept (Schiborr et al., 2013; Sharp & Fonagy, 2008) which refers to a mother’s tendency to treat her child as an individual with an independent mind that is separate from hers (Meins et al., 2001), to see the child as motivated by internal mental states such as desires and feelings (Slade, 2005), and to reflect on her own internal mental states and how they are shaped and changed by interactions with the child (Luyten et al., 2017). Maternal mentalization is thought to promote healthy socioemotional development in children (Zeegers et al., 2017) and has been linked with fewer internalizing and externalizing behaviors among preschoolers (Meins et al., 2013; Smaling et al., 2016). Greater maternal mentalization, particularly mothers’ ability to reflect on their own parenting, has been associated with mothers’ ability to tolerate stress and self-regulate effectively in the face of high levels of distress and physiological arousal (Rutherford et al., 2015).

Mentalization in general, and in the context of parenting in particular, is vulnerable to stress and arousal. It has been argued that with increasing stress there is a “switch point” at which effortful, reflective mentalization collapses, and reflexive, automated, non-reflective thinking takes over (Fonagy and Luyten 2009). However, individuals with strong mentalization skills are able to recover and return to a mentalizing stance after a brief collapse. In contrast, individuals with poor mentalization may respond to stress by prolonged and extensive mentalization collapses, exacerbating their negative arousal and further interfering with their ability to regain reflective, controlled thinking (Fonagy and Luyten, 2009). Thus, mentalization is considered an asset in the face of stress and distress.

Recently it has been argued that maternal mentalization can serve as a moderator: a protective factor that shields children’s development and well-being under adverse circumstances. For instance, higher maternal physiological arousal has been associated with over-controlling behavior in mothers and lower reflective functioning in children, but only among mothers with low mentalization, whereas among mothers who had high mentalization skills, no such associations were found (Borelli et al., 2017). Prior to the pandemic, the current researchers showed that maternal mentalization moderated the link between mothers’ and fathers’ anxiety symptoms and their children’s externalizing behaviors (Dollberg et al., 2021). The moderating effect of maternal mentalization in the face of situational arousal and emotional distress can be attributed to the mother’s ability to recognize her child’s mental and emotional needs (Sharp & Fonagy, 2008), to be reflective regarding her child’s and her own inner experiences and differentiate between the two (Slade, 2005), and consequently to maintain a sensitive parental stance vis-a-vis the child. Because maternal mentalization also involves the ability to self-reflect and appreciate how her (the mother’s) own internal mental states may impact the child, it can also assist mothers in modulating their arousal (Borelli et al., 2017; Rutherford et al., 2015), filtering their own negative emotions (such as anxiety and fear), and self-regulating their negative and insensitive parental responses (Borelli et al., 2017; Ensink et al., 2016). These functions may be especially critical during the pandemic, as children need their parents to recognize, support, and address their fears in a calm way and mediate the overwhelming reality to them in a developmentally appropriate, sensitive, and reassuring fashion (Spinelli et al., 2020). A mother who possesses less advanced mentalization skills, or a mother whose mentalization skills have collapsed due to high stress and anxiety (Luyten et al., 2017), may overburden her child with her own anxieties and fears (Asen & Fonagy, 2012).

In sum, maternal mentalization is considered a parental asset which was shown, prior to the COVID-19 outbreak, to potentially protect children from the negative impact of maternal psychopathology symptoms, and particularly anxiety symptoms. The role of maternal mentalization has not, however, been examined in the context of COVID-19, maternal anxiety symptoms, and child behavior problems. Given that COVID-19 is associated with high levels of maternal anxiety symptoms and children’s distress and fears, it is important to examine whether maternal mentalization might shield children from the negative impact of maternal anxiety symptoms during the current pandemic.

The Current Study

As reviewed above, evidence has accumulated to show an increase in mothers’ anxiety symptoms and children’s socioemotional and behavioral difficulties during the current, ongoing COVID-19 pandemic. However, not much is known about the specific parenting mechanisms that might contribute to this rise in children’s behavioral and emotional problems. Thus, our first objective was to compare reports of maternal anxiety symptoms and children’s behavior problems collected from a group of mothers before the pandemic, and a comparable group of mothers assessed shortly after the pandemic’s outbreak. Next, given the evidence regarding the mediating role of parents’ anxiety in children’s behavior problems in general, and particularly in extreme situations, our second goal was to examine whether maternal anxiety symptoms mediated the link between COVID-19 and children’s reported internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Finally, emerging data have pinpointed maternal mentalization as a parental resource that can modulate the impact of a stressful event on the child directly, as well as by moderating the indirect negative effect of mothers’ anxiety. Thus, our third goal was to test whether maternal mentalization might moderate the direct and indirect links between COVID-19, maternal anxiety symptoms, and children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors.

Study’s Hypotheses

H1: Mothers in the COVID-19 group will report having more anxiety symptoms (H1a) compared to mothers in the pre-COVID-19 group. Mothers in the COVID-19 group will also report their children as having more internalizing and externalizing behaviors (H1b) compared to mothers in the pre-COVID-19 group.

H2: COVID-19 group status will be linked with the child’s externalizing (H2a) and internalizing (H2b) behaviors. The associations between COVID-19 group status and child behaviors will be mediated by the mother’s anxiety symptoms and moderated by the mother’s mentalization skills. Specifically, the direct and indirect links via the mother’s anxiety symptoms will be stronger for mothers with weaker mentalization.

Method

Participants

We tested our hypotheses among a sample comprising 140 Israeli mothers who had a child between 3–6 years of age. The sample consisted of a COVID-19 group (n = 53), recruited during the first wave of the pandemic’s outbreak, and a pre-COVID-19 group (n = 87), of a similar background, recruited prior to the pandemic. Inclusion criteria for both groups were good reported physical health of mothers and children, absence of major developmental difficulties in the child (that required special education), and mothers cohabiting with a spouse/child’s father. For both groups, only mothers who completed all the measures were included in the current analysis. Mothers’ mean age for the entire sample was 35.55 (SD = 4.59). The overall sample consisted of 70 (50%) boys, and the majority of them were firstborn children (91, 64%). Children’s age for the overall sample averaged 4.17 (SD = 0.87). About half of the mothers in the overall sample (72, 50%) reported an above-average household income based on Israeli standards, another 31% (44) reported an average household income, and a minority (25, 17.6%) reported a below-average income. Table 1 presents the demographics of the two groups as well as the comparison among them.

Table 1 Demographics of the COVID-19 and Pre-COVID-19 groups

Procedure

Recruitment for the COVID-19 group was carried out in the midst of the pandemic’s outbreak and during an enforced quarantine in Israel (from mid-March until the end of April 2020). During these months, advertisements were posted on parents’ social networks, and the link to the questionnaire was spread in a snowball-like fashion. Due to the quarantine at that time, the entire procedure was administered online, including the mind-mindedness interview. Data collection ended when the quarantine ended and the school system reopened (with certain restrictions). The study was approved by the Acadmic College of Tel Aviv Yaffo’s Ethics Committee (ref. 2020079). Mothers from the pre-COVID-19 group were similarly recruited through posts on social networks and the snowball method. Mothers completed online questionnaires and were interviewed and audiotaped with the mind-mindedness assessment procedure during a home visit. Mothers from both groups were asked to consider only one of their children in the specified age range when completing the questionnaires. All participants signed an informed consent prior to participation, participation was anonymous and voluntary, and there was no reward or compensation for participation.

Measures

Mothers' Anxiety Symptomatology

We used the 6-item anxiety subscale of the Brief Symptom Index (BSI; Derogatis, 1993) to assess each mother’s reported anxiety symptoms. The BSI is a widely used, psychometrically validated, reliable self-report questionnaire (Derogatis & Melisaratos, 1983), comprising 53 items that measure distress. Respondents were asked to answer questions such as “Over the last month, to what extent did you feel fearful?" on a 5-point Likert-type scale, ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (extremely). Higher scores reflect greater psychopathology symtpoms. Internal consistency of the anxiety subscale in the current study was found to be good for both the pre-COVID-19 and the COVID-19 group (α = 0.77, 0.78 respectively).

Maternal Mentalization

We utilized Meins and colleagues’ (see Meins & Fernyhough, 2010) mind-mindedness (MM) procedure to assess mothers’ mentalization representational skills. Mothers were asked: "Can you describe [child] for me?" The mothers were told that there were no right or wrong answers and that they could describe their children in any way they wished. For mothers in the pre-COVID-19 group, the mind-mindedness procedure was conducted by interviews that were recorded and transcribed verbatim prior to coding. Mothers in the COVID-19 group were asked to write online their response to "Can you describe [child] for me?" For both groups, coding consisted of identifying comments that included internal-state terms referring to the child’s mind, such as references to the child’s wishes, desires, mental states (e.g., thoughts, knowledge, interests), mental processes (e.g., recognition, remembering, decision-making), emotions, and attempts to manipulate people’s beliefs (e.g., joking, teasing). Next, the rate of these statements (mind-mindedness statements) out of the total number of statements provided by the mother was calculated, and this figure represented the mother’s representational mentalization (mind-mindedness) level. Two graduate students trained by the first author coded the material. Interrater agreement during coding was tested on 10% of randomly selected interviews in each group and was good for both (pre-COVID-19 kappa = 0.85, COVID-19 kappa = 0.94).

Children's Behavior Problems

The Child Behavioral Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Rescorla, 2000) for children (1.5- 5 years old version) was used to evaluate the child’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors as perceived by the mother. The CBCL is a widely used 99-item questionnaire describing child behavior during the past months. Items are presented on a 3-point Likert-type scale, with 0 = not true, 1 = somewhat or sometimes true, and 2 = very or often true. Item examples include "afraid to try new things" and "can't sit still, is restless, or hyperactive." The CBCL produces two clusters: the internalizing behavior cluster and the externalizing behavior cluster. In the current study both clusters demonstrated good internal consistency in the pre-COVID-19 group (internalizing: α = 0.82; externalizing: α = 0.86) as well as the COVID-19 group (internalizing: α = 0.88; externalizing: α = 0.89).

Data Analysis Strategy

We first compared the COVID-19 and pre-COVID-19 groups in terms of demographics. We then computed Pearson’s r correlations to investigate zero-order associations among the study’s variables, and between them and the participants’ demographics. To test H1 we used ANCOVA. To test H2, we used the PROCESS version 3.0 macro (Hayes, 2018) model 59 for testing moderation and mediation effects with 5,000 bias-corrected bootstrap samples (Hayes, 2018). PROCESS allowed us to combine mediation and moderation within a single model and estimate the conditional and unconditional direct and indirect effects. We ran the analyses for the child’s externalizing and internalizing behaviors separately, resulting in two models. In each model we tested the direct and indirect links between COVID-19 group status (pre-COVID-19 vs. COVID-19) and child’s externalizing/internalizing behaviors, mediated via mothers’ anxiety and moderated by mothers’ mind-mindedness, while controlling for possible demographic confounders. We considered the effects significant at p < 0.05. When the 95% confidence interval included 0, we inferred a significant indirect effect at the 0.05 level. Given the relatively small sample size, we applied the bias-corrected bootstrap samples method (Hayes & Preacher, 2013). All analyses were performed with SPSS version 25 (IBM Corp, 2017).

Results

First, we compared the COVID-19 and pre-COVID-19 groups in terms of demographics. The results are presented in Table 1. Accordingly, we found that children in the pre-COVID-19 group were younger and, relatedly, tended to attend private (rather than public) schools. Furthermore, “below-average” household income was more prevalent in the pre-COVID-19 group than in the COVID-19 group. Next, we examined zero-order correlations between the variable in which the groups differed (i.e., child’s age, education setting, and household income) and the study’s variables. This procedure revealed significant correlations between “child’s age” (r = 0.31, p < 0.001) and “child’s education setting” (r = -0.25. p < 0.01) and mothers’ mind-mindedness, so that mothers of older children and those attending public schools (and therefore likely to be older) displayed higher mind-mindedness. Household income did not correlate with any of the study’s variables. Therefore “child’s age” and “education setting” were included in the analyses where mind-mindedness was tested.

Next, we examined intercorrelations between the study’s variables for the entire sample and within each group. Table 2 presents the study’s variables’ means, SDs, and intercorrelations for the entire sample and for each group. As shown in Table 2, we found strong positive correlations between the mothers’ anxiety symptom scores and their reports of their children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors for the entire sample and within each group. Maternal mentalization did not correlate with either maternal anxiety or child behavior problems.

Table 2 Means, SDs, and intercorrelations between the study’s variables for the entire sample (N = 140) and by group

In support of H1, the multivariate analysis showed an overall significant between-group difference, F (3, 136) = 6.30, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.12. Univariate analysis revealed the expected differences between the groups, so that mothers in the COVID-19 group reported significantly more anxiety symptoms (H1a), F (1, 138) = 18.97, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.12, and more child internalizing, F (1, 138) = 4.63, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.03, and externalizing behaviors (H1b), F (1, 138) = 4.62, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.03, compared to mothers in the pre-COVID-19 group. Thus, hypotheses H1a and H1b were fully supported.

We tested H2 with two models, one predicting child’s externalizing and the other predicting child’s internalizing behaviors. Power analysis using G*Power3.1.9.2 software showed that the sample size was sufficient for detecting a medium-sized (ƒ2 = 0.35) mediation-moderation effect with a probability of almost 100%. As hypothesized, the model predicting child’s externalizing behaviors (H2a) was significant, F (7, 132) = 10.12, p < 0.001 R2 = 0.35. Figure 1 presents the direct and the mediated moderated links between the variables. The model was tested with child’s age and education setting as covariates; however, for the sake of visual clarity, these covariates are not presented in the figure. Accordingly, mothers’ anxiety fully mediated the link between COVID-19 group status and child’s externalizing behaviors. Furthermore, a significant interaction effect between mothers’ mind-mindedness and mothers’ anxiety was found. Indirect links were tested at three points of mind-mindedness: low mind-mindedness (1 SD below average), medium mind-mindedness (average), and high mind-mindedness (1 SD above average). Table 3 presents the conditional indirect effects at all three points. As can be seen in Table 3, significant indirect effects were found at all three points. Pairwise contrast tests showed that the differences in effects between the three points of mind-mindedness were significant. Figure 2 provides a visual demonstration of the conditional effects of the focal predictor on the values of the moderator as produced by the PROCESS script. As indicated in Table 3 and visually presented in Fig. 2, the strongest association between mother’s anxiety symptoms and child’s externalizing behaviors was when mother’s mind-mindedness was low, whereas when mother’s mind-mindedness was higher, the association was weaker. In sum, hypothesis H2a was fully supported.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Mediated moderation model of COVID-19 group status predicting child’s externalizing behaviors via mother’s anxiety symptoms moderated by mother’s mind-mindedness. Note: Numbers on lines are unstandardized path coefficients; ** p < .01, ***p < .001

Table 3 Conditional Indirect Effects of COVID-19 Group Status Mediated by Mother’s Anxiety Symptoms on Child’s Externalizing and Internalizing Behaviors
Fig. 2
figure 2

Associations between mother’s anxiety symptoms and child’s externalizing behaviors by mother’s mind-mindedness level. Note: Low Mind-Mindedness = 1SD above Mean; Medium Mind-Mindedness = Mean; High Mind-Mindedness = 1SD above Mean

The model predicting child’s internalizing behaviors (H2b) was also significant, F (7, 132) = 6.15, p < 0.001 R2 = 0.25. Mothers’ anxiety similarly fully mediated the link between COVID-19 group status and child’s internalizing behaviors at all points of mothers’ mind-mindedness. Table 3 presents the conditional indirect effects at all three points. Accordingly, significant indirect effects were found at all three points of mothers’ mind-mindedness. However, contrary to our hypothesis, no interaction effect of mothers’ mind-mindedness on either the direct or indirect links was found. Thus, hypothesis H2b was only partially supported.

Discussion

Our findings join a rapidly emerging and greatly needed body of research documenting the increase in adult and children’s psychopathology symptoms in response to the current pandemic. Specifically, our study showed that during COVID-19, mothers of preschool children experienced more anxiety symptoms and perceived their children as having more externalizing and internalizing behaviors than did mothers of children of similar ages and background who were interviewed before the COVID-19 outbreak. Moreover, our findings indicated that, as hypothesized, mothers’ anxiety symptoms mediated the effect of the pandemic on children’s behavior problems so that the higher mothers’ anxiety was, the more externalizing and internalizing behaviors were reported by the mothers about their children. Finally, the findings supported our hypothesis that mothers’ mind-mindedness moderated the association between the pandemic and children’s externalizing behaviors. Specifically, when mothers showed higher mentalization skills (higher mind-mindedness), the indirect effect of anxiety symptoms on the link between COVID-19 and child’s externalizing behaviors was weaker than when mothers showed lower mentalization skills (lower mind-mindedness).

The finding regarding higher rates of anxiety symptoms among mothers and more reports of behavior problems in their children during COVID-19 compared to reports by mothers interviewed prior to the pandemic is consistent with mounting evidence from around the world documenting the negative socioemotional impact of the pandemic on adults and particularly on mothers (e.g., Dubey et al., 2020) and on their children (Davenport et al., 2020; Domínguez-Álvarez et al., 2020; Jiao et al., 2020; Malhotra et al., 2020; Orgilés et al., 2020). The increase in mothers’ anxiety symptoms and children’s socioemotional and behavioral difficulties are understandable, given the objective and subjective risks associated with the pandemic, and the health, economic, and social crises accompanying it. Of note is the fact that our data were collected shortly after the World Health Organization (WHO) declared that the COVID-19 outbreak constituted a pandemic, leading Israel, like other countries, to enforce a countrywide quarantine, shutting down schools, workplaces, playgrounds, and recreation sites, and limiting social and extended family gatherings. Moreover, initially the medical establishment and the public had a very limited understanding of the disease, the mechanisms by which it spread, and the ways in which to manage and treat it. The ambiguity and sense of threat that surrounded the pandemic may have led mothers, and particularly mothers of young children, to feel overwhelmed, fearful, and helpless, all possible contributors to the increased anxiety among mothers and the socioemotional difficulties among their children (Spielberger, 2013).

Accumulating data indicate that children’s malevolent or pathological responses to external crises, and particularly the responses of younger children, are closely tied to their caregivers’ reactions to the crises (Juth et al., 2015). Our findings provide further support for this argument by showing that, as hypothesized, mothers’ anxiety symptoms mediated the negative link between COVID-19 and children’s elevated internalizing and externalizing behaviors. In other words, it was not the pandemic itself that accounted for the increase in children’s externalizing and internalizing behaviors. Rather, the pandemic seemed to lead to mothers’ experience of elevated anxiety symptoms, which, in turn, seemed to increase the risk of more externalizing and internalizing behaviors in their children. Supporting this idea, recent reports have indicated that mothers’ anxiety and stress during the pandemic has interfered with parenting (Romero et al., 2020), leading mothers to demonstrate more hostile and coercive parenting (Chung et al., 2020) which, in turn, may increase the risk of more externalizing and internalizing behaviors among their children.

Consistent with our hypothesis, mothers’ mentalization skills, operationalized via the mind-mindedness construct, moderated the negative link between their anxiety symptoms and their children’s externalizing behaviors. Specifically, as hypothesized, mothers who demonstrated lower mentalization skills and higher anxiety symptoms also reported more child externalizing behaviors. In contrast, when the mother demonstrated better mentalization skills, she also reported less anxiety symptoms and less externalizing behaviors in her child. Of note is the fact that maternal mentalization was unrelated to mothers’ anxiety symptoms; it also did not moderate the direct link between COVID-19 group status and child’s externalizing and internalizing behaviors. Taken together, these findings suggest that although maternal mentalization may not spare mothers from experiencing anxiety or undo the negative effects of COVID-19 on mothers and children, it can reduce the risk of children developing behavior problems despite the potentially negative impact upon them of their mothers’ anxiety symptoms. Given these findings and what we know about how parental mentalization works, we can suggest, albeit tentatively, specific mechanisms by which mentalization may exert its buffering effect on children during the current pandemic. Accordingly, higher maternal mentalization helps the mother to “step into her child’s shoes” (Meins et al., 2001), differentiate between her own and her child’s mind (Sharp & Fonagy, 2008), and appreciate her child’s limited developmental capacity to comprehend complex situations (Slade, 2005). This understanding can lead mothers with higher mentalization skills to appreciate the potentially overwhelming effect of their own anxiety on their young children and therefore try to conceal and modulate it. In terms of the current study’s findings, mothers in the COVID-19 group with higher mentalization skills may have tried and been successful in moderating their own anxiety when interacting with their young children and, by doing so, provided their children with a sense of safety and assurance. In contrast, mothers with lower mentalization skills may have shared their worries with their children, overburdening and confusing them and thus increasing the risk of behavior problems in their children. These mothers may have discussed frightening information in the presence of the children or overwhelmed them with media reports, failing to appreciate how these actions may have affected the children emotionally. This explanation is in accord with earlier findings reported prior to the pandemic, indicating that mothers who are characterized by higher mentalization skills are better at recognizing and modulating their negative emotions (Ensink et al., 2016) and physiological arousal (Rutherford et al., 2015) when stressed. Furthermore, mothers with higher mentalization skills may be more tolerant and accepting of their children’s negative behaviors, and particularly externalizing behaviors, because they look for and recognize the inner experiences that lie beneath the behaviors and act as affect regulators for their children (Gergely et al., 2002). In contrast, mothers with lower mentalization skills may fail to understand the underlying motives beneath their children’s behavior, particularly when it comes to disturbing behaviors. Indeed, recent reports have indicated that during the pandemic, many children have reacted with more problematic behaviors (Jiao et al., 2020; Orgilés et al, 2020; Romero et al., 2020). Thus, mothers with weaker mentalization skills may have reacted more negatively to their children’s externalizing behaviors, failing to anticipate, contain, and understand them, which may have further increased their own distress, sense of helplessness, and anxiety. Thus, a vicious circle may emerge in which mothers characterized by more anxiety symptoms and lower mentalization skills may face more disturbing externalizing behaviors in their children which, in turn, may further add to the mother’s already high anxiety symptoms.

Unexpectedly, we did not find a moderating effect of maternal mentalization in regard to internalizing behaviors, despite a mediating effect of maternal anxiety symptoms on the link between COVID-19 and child’s internalizing behaviors. These results accord with those of a Colonnesi and colleagues’ (2019) study, in which they were unable to find the predicted association between parental mind-mindedness and preschoolers’ internalizing behaviors. The authors attributed the lack of significant findings to the fact that internalized behaviors are less apparent behaviorally in preschoolers and are therefore less visible to parents. Similarly, we would argue that the kind of symptomatology associated with internalizing behaviors (i.e., fears, withdrawal, and avoidance) may be perceived by mothers, regardless of their mentalization skills, as more normative and acceptable during the preschool years in general and in the context of the pandemic in particular. Finally, the lack of a significant moderation effect may be due to the size of the sample and limited statistical power.

Strengths, Limitations, and Future Studies

Our study’s strengths lie in comparing – via self-report questionnaires and an open-ended, semi-structured measurement – mothers’ and children’s socioemotional functioning before and during the COVID-19 outbreak. In addition, it offers and tests a conceptual model to explain the increase as well as variations in COVID19-related symptomatology among mothers and children. Its value lies in the comparison of two groups of mothers and children of similar backgrounds assessed by the same research tools during two significantly different times: before and during the pandemic. However, there were several limitations, including a relatively modest sample size, concurrent data collection in each group, and a reliance on mothers as a source of information. These factors limited our ability to detect additional effects and to determine causality. For example, the increase in mothers’ reports of their own anxiety symptoms and their children’s elevated behavior problems may at least in part have been a reflection of the media’s extensive reports of such phenomena, potentially priming mothers to have a heightened or inaccurate awareness of them, rather than being a reflection of a real, objective increase in such behaviors. Additional limitations included focusing on heterosexual mothers who share household and childcare responsibilities with a spouse, and collecting data electronically, limiting the generalizability of the findings to other, more at-risk populations. Furthermore, the study’s procedure did not allow us to assess the contribution of fathers’ and/or dyadic influences in the context of children and families coping with the pandemic. Finally, given home confinement and social distancing regulations during the recruitment of the COVID-19 group, all data collection was carried out via the internet. In contrast, in the pre-COVID-19 group, the mind-mindedness interview procedure was conducted in person. This, as well as other group differences might better explain the current findings. Future studies should use longitudinal designs, following parents (both mothers and fathers) who were recruited prior to the pandemic and assessing relevant parental reports during the pandemic and in the future, after the pandemic is hopefully contained. Future studies should also expand the assessment procedures, assessing children’s and parents’ symptoms of psychopathology and socioemotional distress via other sources (e.g., teachers’ reports and clinical interviews). Finally, we used the mind-mindedness construct to assess maternal mentalization. The mind-mindedness construct assesses a particular aspect of mentalization, namely the mother’s tendency to view her child as a mental agent. Thus, the role of other aspects of maternal mentalization such as parental reflective functioning could not be addressed by this study. Moreover, it has been argued that mothers who demonstrate high off-line, interview-derived maternal mentalization skills do not necessarily demonstrate the same skills when interacting with their children directly (Shai et al., 2017). Thus, future studies should use other methods of assessing parental mentalization to fully understand its many dimensions.

Conclusions and Clinical Implications

The COVID-19 pandemic is an ongoing, global crisis with extensive health, economic, social, and psychological implications for children and their parents that are only beginning to be explored and understood. Our study’s results underscore the risk associated with maternal anxiety symptomatology for children’s behavior problems when mothers are overwhelmed and fail to contain their distress. On a more optimistic note, the findings also indicate that mothers’ mentalization skills may serve as a source of resilience, protecting children from the risk associated with maternal anxiety. This point is especially important at the current time, when mothers are called on to help their children overcome the emotional and behavioral difficulties linked to the pandemic and its psychological sequelae. Drawing on our findings, we can argue, albeit tentatively, that supporting mothers by helping them to manage and contain their anxiety symptoms can help decrease signs of psychopathology in both mothers and their young children. Thus, encouraging mothers to reduce their anxiety levels by making time for themselves to exercise, practice relaxation and mindfulness techniques, and maintain social connections, even if only remotely, is crucial. Mothers might also benefit from developmental guidance and psychoeducation regarding young children’s reactions to threats and can learn techniques to better manage children’s negative reactions to the current pandemic. Furthermore, the findings provide support for our assertion that maternal mentalization is an important parental resource. There are several evidence-based therapeutic interventions that work to enhance parental mentalization during the preschool years (e.g., Huber et al., 2015; Ross et al., 2020; Suchman et al., 2010). These may be adapted and utilized as preventive and therapeutic interventions to enhance maternal mentalization among mothers/parents coping with the pandemic and its aftermath.