Introduction

In our daily lives, our dominant perception is of order. But every now and then chaos threatens that order: epidemics dramatically break out, revolutions erupt, empires suddenly fall, and stock markets crash. Epidemics, in particular, present not only collective health hazards but also special challenges to mental health and public order that need to be addressed by social and behavioral sciences (Van Bavel et al., 2020). Almost 30 years ago, in the wake of the AIDS epidemic, Philip Strong, the founder of the sociological study of epidemic infectious diseases, reflected: “the human origin of epidemic psychology lies not so much in our unruly passions as in the threat of epidemic disease to our everyday assumptions” (Strong, 1990). In the recent COVID-19 pandemic (Brooks et al., 2020) (an ongoing pandemic of a coronavirus disease), it has been shown that the main source of uncertainty and anxiety has indeed come from the disruption of what Alfred Shutz called the “routines and recipes” of daily life (Schutz et al., 1973) (e.g., every simple act, from eating at work to visiting our parents, takes on new meanings).

Yet, the chaos resulting from an epidemic turns out to be more predictable than what one would initially expect. Philip Strong observed that any new health epidemic resulted into three psycho-social epidemics: of fear, moralization, and action. The epidemic of fear represents the fear of catching the disease, which comes with the suspicion against alleged disease carriers, which, in turn, may spark panic and irrational behavior. The epidemic of moralization is characterized by moral responses both to the viral epidemic itself and to the epidemic of fear, which may result in either positive reactions (e.g., cooperation) or negative ones (e.g., stigmatization). The epidemic of action accounts for the rational or irrational changes of daily habits that people make in response to the disease or as a result of the two other psycho-social epidemics. Strong was writing in the wake of the AIDS/HIV crisis, but he based his model on studies that went back to Europe’s Black Death in the 14th century. Importantly, he showed that these three psycho-social epidemics are created by language and incrementally fed through it: language transmits the fear that the infection is an existential threat to humanity and that we are all going to die; language depicts the epidemic as a verdict on human failings and as a moral judgment on minorities; and language shapes the means through which people collectively intend to, however pointless, act against the threat.

There have been numerous studies of how information propagated on social media during epidemic outbreaks occurred in the past decade, such as Zika (Fu et al., 2016; Sommariva et al., 2018; Wood, 2018), Ebola (Oyeyemi et al., 2014), and the H1N1 influenza (Chew and Eysenbach, 2010). Similarly, with the outbreak of COVID-19, people around the world have been collectively expressing their thoughts and concerns about the pandemic on social media. As such, researchers studied this epidemic from multiple angles: social media posts have been analyzed in terms of content and behavioral markers (Hou, 2020; Li et al., 2020), and of tracking the diffusion of COVID-related information (Cinelli et al., 2020) and misinformation (Ferrara, 2020; Kouzy et al., 2020; Pulido et al., 2020; Yang et al., 2020). Search queries have suggested specific information-seeking responses to the pandemic (Bento et al., 2020). Psychological responses to COVID-19 have been studied mostly though surveys (Qiu et al., 2020; Wang et al., 2020).

Hitherto, there has never been any large-scale empirical study of whether the use of language during an epidemic reflects Strong’s model. With the opportunity of having a sufficiently large-scale data at hand, we set out to test whether Strong’s model did hold on Twitter during COVID-19, and did so at the scale of an entire country, that of the United States. By running our study on Twitter, we expose the interpretation of our results to a number of limitations, notably to issues of representativeness (Li et al., 2013), self-presentation biases (Waterloo et al., 2018), and data noise (Ferrara et al., 2016; Saif et al., 2012). Indeed, recent surveys estimated that only 22% of U.S. adults use Twitter (Wojcik and Hughes, 2019), and that the characteristics of these users deviate from the general population’s: compared to the average adult in the U.S., Twitter users are much younger, are more likely to have college degrees, and are slightly more likely to identify with the Democratic Party. Despite such limitations, social media represents the response of a relevant part of the general population to global events, and it does so at a scale and a granularity that have been so far unattainable by publicly available data sources.

After operationalizing Strong’s model by using lexicons for psycholinguistic text analysis, upon the collection of 122M tweets about the epidemic from 1 February to 31 December, we conducted a quantitative analysis on the differences in language style and a thematic analysis of the actual social media posts. The temporal scope of our study does not capture the full pandemic as it is still ongoing at the time of writing but it includes the three major contagion waves in the U.S., characterizing the entire year of 2020. The first wave captured the initial diffusion of the virus in the world, and then its arrival and first peak in the U.S. The following two waves captured subsequent periods of alarming diffusion.

The three psycho-social epidemics, as theorized by Strong, evolve concurrently over time. In the particular case of our period of study, we found that this concurrent evolution resulted into three regimes or phases, which are not part of Strong’s theoretical framework and experimentally emerged. In the first phase (the refusal phase), the psycho-social epidemic of fear began. Twitter users in the U.S. refused to accept reality: they feared the uncertainty created by the disruption of what was considered to be “normal”; focused their moral concerns on others in an act of distancing oneself from others; yet, despite all this, they refused to change the normal course of action. After the announcement of the first death in the country, the second phase (the anger phase) began: the psycho-social epidemic of fear intensified while the epidemics of morality and action kicked-off abruptly. Twitter users expressed more anger than fear about the looming feeling that things were about to change; focused their moral concerns on oneself in an act of reckoning with what was happening; and suspended their daily activities. After the authorities imposed physical-distancing measures, the third phase (the acceptance phase) took over: the epidemic of fear started to fade away while the epidemics of morality and action turned into more constructive and forward-looking social processes. Twitter users expressed more sadness than anger or fear; focused their moral concerns on the collective and, in so doing, promoted pro-social behavior; and found a “new normal” in their daily activities, which consisted of their past daily activities being physically restricted to their homes and neighborhoods. The phase of acceptance dominated Twitter conversations for the rest of the year, although the anger phase re-emerged cyclically with the rise of new waves of contagion. In particular, we observed two peaks of anger: when the death toll in the U.S. reached 100,000 people (second contagion wave), and when President Trump tested positive to COVID-19 (third contagion wave).

Dataset

From an existing collection of COVID-related tweets (Chen et al., 2020), we gathered 554,941,519 tweets posted between 1 February up to 31 December. We focused our analysis on the United States, the country where Twitter penetration is highest. To identify Twitter users living in it, we parsed the free-text location description of their user profile (e.g., “San Francisco, CA”). We did so by using a set of custom regular expressions that match variations for the expression “United States of America”, as well as the names of 333 cities and 51 states in the U.S. (and their combinations). Albeit not always accurate, matching location strings against known location names is a tested approach that yields good results for a coarse-grained localization at state or country-level (Dredze et al., 2013). Overall, we were left with 6,271,835 unique users in the U.S. who posted 122,320,155 tweets in English.

Before analyzing how our language categories unfolded over time, we experimentally tested whether the number of data points at hand was sufficient to compute our metrics. It was indeed the case as the number of active users per day varied from a minimum of 72k on 2 February to a maximum of 1.84M on 18 March, with and average of 437k. A small number of accounts tweeted a disproportionately high number of times, reaching a maximum of 15,823 tweets; those were clearly automated accounts, which were discarded by our methodology. As we shall discuss in Methods, we normalized all our aggregate temporal measures so that they were not affected by the fluctuating volume of tweets over time.

Methods

Coding Strong’s model

Back in the 1990s, Philip Strong was able not only to describe the psychological impact of epidemics on social order but also to model it. He observed that the early reaction to major fatal epidemics is a distinctive psycho-social form and can be modeled along three main dimensions: fear, morality, and action. During a large-scale epidemic, basic assumptions about social interaction and, more generally, about social order are disrupted, and, more specifically, they are so by: the fear of others, competing moralities, and the responses to the epidemic. Crucially, all these three elements are created, transmitted, and mediated by language: language transmits fears, elaborates on the stigmatization of minorities, and shapes the means through which people collectively respond to the epidemic (Cap, 2016; Goffman, 2009; Strong, 1990).

As opposed to existing attempts to model psychological and social aspects of epidemic crises (Khan and Huremović, 2019; McConnell, 2005), Strong’s model meets our three main choice criteria:

  1. (1)

    Well-grounded: It proposes a comprehensive, highly cited, and still relevant theoretical model, which is based on an extensive review of studies of past large-scale epidemics that did span centuries and, as such, were of different nature, speaking to the generalizability of the framework.

  2. (2)

    Focused on psycho-social aspects: Its main goal is to characterize people’s psychological and social responses to epidemics rather than describing how an epidemic unfolds over time.

  3. (3)

    Directly operationalizable from language use: The description of the psycho-social responses provided by Strong lends itself to operationalization, as its defining concepts have been mapped to language markers by previous literature (as Table 1 shows).

    Table 1 Operationalization of the Strong’s epidemic psychology theoretical framework.

We operationalized Strong’s epidemic psychology theoretical framework in two steps. First, three authors hand-coded Strong’s seminal paper (Strong, 1990) using line-by-line coding (Gibbs, 2007) to identify keywords that characterize the three psycho-social epidemics. For each of the three psycho-social epidemics, the three authors generated independent lists of keywords that were conservatively combined by intersecting them. The words that were left out by the intersection were mostly synonyms (e.g., “catching disease” as a synonym for “contagion”), so we did not discard any important concept. According to Strong, the three psycho-social epidemics are intertwined and, as such, the concepts that define one specific psycho-social epidemic might be relevant to the remaining two as well. For example, suspicion is an element of the epidemic of fear but is tightly related to stigmatization as well, a phenomenon that Strong describes as typical of the epidemic of moralization. In our coding exercise, we adhered as much as possible to the description in Strong’s paper and obtained a strict partition of keywords across psycho-social epidemics. In the second step, the same three authors mapped each of these keywords to language categories, namely sets of words that reflect how these concepts are expressed in natural language (e.g., words expressing anger or trust). We took these categories from four existing language lexicons widely used in psychometric studies:

  • Linguistic Inquiry Word Count (LIWC) (Tausczik and Pennebaker, 2010): A lexicon of words and word stems grouped into over 125 categories reflecting emotions, social processes, and basic functions, among others. The LIWC lexicon is based on the premise that the words people use to communicate can provide clues to their psychological states (Tausczik and Pennebaker, 2010). It allows written passages to be analyzed syntactically (how the words are used together to form phrases or sentences) and semantically (an analysis of the meaning of the words or phrases).

  • Emolex (Mohammad and Turney, 2013): A lexicon that classifies 6k+ words and stems into the eight primary emotions of Plutchik’s psychoevolutionary theory (Plutchik, 1991).

  • Moral Foundation Lexicon (Graham et al., 2009): A lexicon of 318 words and stems, which are grouped into five categories of moral foundations (Graham et al., 2013): harm, fairness, in-group, authority, and purity. Each of which is further split into expressions of virtue or vice.

  • Pro-social behavior (Frimer et al., 2014): A lexicon of 146 pro-social words and stems, which have been found to be frequently used when people describe pro-social goals (Frimer et al., 2014).

The three authors grouped similar keywords together and mapped groups of keywords to one or more language categories. This grouping and mapping procedure was informed by previous studies that investigated how these keywords are expressed through language. These studies are listed in Table 1.

Language categories over time

We considered that a tweet contained a language category c if at least one of the tweet’s words or stems belonged to that category. The tweet-category association is binary and disregards the number of matching words within the same tweet. That is mainly because, in short snippets of text (tweets are limited to 280 characters), multiple occurrences are rare and do not necessarily reflect the intensity of a category (Russell, 2013). For each language category c, we counted the number of users Uc(t) who posted at least one tweet at time t containing that category. We then obtained the fraction of users who mentioned category c by dividing Uc(t) by the total number of users U(t) who tweeted at time t:

$${f}_{c}(t)=\frac{{U}_{c}(t)}{U(t)}.$$
(1)

Computing the fraction of users rather than the fraction of tweets prevents biases introduced by exceptionally active users, thus capturing more faithfully the prevalence of different language categories in our Twitter population. This also helps discounting the impact of social bots, which tend to have anomalous levels of activity (especially retweeting (Bessi and Ferrara, 2016)).

Different categories might be verbalized with considerably different frequencies. For example, the language category “I” (first-person pronoun) from the LIWC lexicon naturally occurred much more frequently than the category “death” from the same lexicon. To enable a comparison across categories, we standardized all the fractions:

$${z}_{c}(t)=\frac{{f}_{c}(t)-{\mu }_{[0,T]}({f}_{c})}{{\sigma }_{[0,T]}({f}_{c})},$$
(2)

where μ(fc) and σ(fc) represent the mean and standard deviation of the fc(t) scores over the whole time period, from t = 0 (1 February) to t = T (16 April). These z-scores ease also the interpretation of the results as they represent the relative variation of a category’s prevalence compared to its average: they take on values higher (lower) than zero when the original value is higher (lower) than the average.

Other behavioral markers

To assess the validity of our operationalization of Strong’s model, we compared its results with the output of alternative state-of-the-art text-mining techniques, and with real-world mobility patterns.

Interaction types

We compared the results obtained via word-matching with a state-of-the-art deep learning tool for Natural Language Processing designed to capture fundamental types of social interactions from conversational language (Choi et al., 2020). This tool uses Long Short-Term Memory neural networks (LSTMs) (Hochreiter and Schmidhuber, 1997) that take in input a 300-dimensional GloVe representation of words (Pennington et al., 2014) and output a series of confidence scores in the range [0, 1] that estimate the likelihood that the text expresses certain types of social interactions. The classifiers exhibited a very high classification performance, up to an Area Under the ROC Curve (AUC) of 0.98. AUC is a performance metric that measures the ability of the model to assign higher confidence scores to positive examples (i.e., text characterized by the type of interaction of interest) than to negative examples, independent of any fixed decision threshold; the expected value for random classification is 0.5, whereas an AUC of 1 indicates a perfect classification.

Out of the 10 interaction types that the tool can classify (Deri et al., 2018), three were detected frequently with likelihood > 0.5 in our Twitter data: conflict (expressions of contrast or diverging views (Tajfel et al., 1979)), social support (giving emotional or practical aid and companionship (Fiske et al., 2007)), and power (expressions that mark a person’s power over the behavior and outcomes of another (Blau, 1964)).

Given a tweet’s textual message m and an interaction type i, we used the classifier to compute the likelihood score li(m) that the message contains that interaction type. We then binarized the confidence scores using a threshold-based indicator function:

$${l}_{i}^{\theta }(m)=\left\{\begin{array}{ll}1,&\,\text{if}\,\,{l}_{i}{\it(m)}\ge {\theta }_{i}\\ 0,&\,\text{otherwise}\,\end{array}\right.$$
(3)

Following the original approach (Choi et al., 2020), we used a different threshold for each interaction type, as the distributions of their likelihood scores tend to vary considerably. We thus picked conservatively θi as the value of the 85th percentile of the distribution of the confidence scores li, thus favoring precision over recall. Last, similar to how we constructed temporal signals for the language categories, we counted the number of users Ui(t) who posted at least one tweet at time t that contains interaction type i. We then obtained the fraction of users who mentioned interaction type i by dividing Ui(t) by the total number of users U(t) who tweeted at time t:

$${f}_{i}(t)=\frac{{U}_{i}(t)}{U(t)}.$$
(4)

Last, we min−max normalized these fractions, considering the minimum and maximum values during the whole time period [0, T]:

$${f}_{i}(t)=\frac{{f}_{i}(t)-\mathop{\min }\limits_{[0,T]}({f}_{i})}{\mathop{\max }\limits_{[0,T]}({f}_{i})-\mathop{\min }\limits_{[0,T]}({f}_{i})}.$$
(5)

Mentions of medical entities

We used a state-of-the-art deep learning method for medical entity extraction to identify medical symptoms on Twitter in relation to COVID-19 (Scepanovic et al., 2020). When applied to tweets, the method extracts n-grams representing medical symptoms (e.g., “feeling sick”). This method is based on the Bi-LSTM sequence-tagging architecture (Huang et al., 2015) in combination with GloVe word embeddings (Pennington et al., 2014) and RoBERTa contextual embeddings (Liu et al., 2019). To optimize the entity extraction performance on noisy textual data from social media, we trained its sequence-tagging architecture on the Micromed database (Jimeno-Yepes et al., 2015), a collection of tweets manually labeled with medical entities. The hyper-parameters we used are: 256 hidden units, a batch size of 4, and a learning rate of 0.1 which we gradually halved whenever there was no performance improvement after 3 epochs. We trained for a maximum of 200 epochs or before the learning rate became too small (≤0.0001). The final model achieved an F1-score of 0.72 on Micromed. The F1-score is a performance measure that combines precision (the fraction of extracted entities that are actually medical entities) and recall (the fraction of medical entities present in the text that the method is able to retrieve). We based our implementation on Flair (Akbik et al., 2019) and Pytorch (Paszke et al., 2017), two popular deep learning libraries in Python.

For each unique medical entity e, we counted the number of users Ue(t) who posted at least one tweet at time t that mentioned that entity. We then obtained the fraction of users who mentioned medical entity e by dividing Ue(t) by the total number of users U(t) who tweeted at time t:

$${f}_{e}(t)=\frac{{U}_{e}(t)}{U(t)}.$$
(6)

Last, we min–max normalize these fractions, considering the minimum and maximum values during the whole time period [0, T]:

$${f}_{e}(t)=\frac{{f}_{e}(t)-\mathop{\min }\limits_{[0,T]}({f}_{e})}{\mathop{\max }\limits_{[0,T]}({f}_{e})-\mathop{\min }\limits_{[0,T]}({f}_{e})}.$$
(7)

Mobility traces

Foursquare is a local search and discovery mobile application that relies on the users’ past mobility records to recommend places user might may like. The application uses GPS geo-localization to estimate the user position and to infer the places they visited. In response to the COVID-19 crisis, Foursquare made publicly available the data gathered from a pool of 13 million U.S. users. These users were “always-on” during the period of data collection, meaning that they allowed the application to gather geo-location data at all times, even when the application was not in use. The data (published through the visitdata.orgwebsite) consists of the daily number of users vs,j visiting any venue of type j in state s, starting from 1 February to the present day (e.g., 419,256 users visited schools in Indiana on 1 February). Overall, 35 distinct location categories were provided. To obtain country-wide temporal indicators, we first applied a min-max normalization to the vs,j values:

$${v}_{s,j}(t)=\frac{{v}_{s,j}(t)-\mathop{\min }\limits_{[0,T]}({v}_{s,j})}{\mathop{\max }\limits_{[0,T]}({v}_{s,j})-\mathop{\min }\limits_{[0,T]}({v}_{s,j})}.$$
(8)

We then averaged the values across all states:

$${v}_{j}(t)=\frac{1}{S}{\sum }_{s}{v}_{s,j}(t),$$
(9)

where S is the total number of states. By weighting each state equally, we obtained a measure that is more representative of the whole U.S. territory, rather than being biased towards high-density regions.

Time series smoothing

All our temporal indicators are affected by large day-to-day fluctuations. To extract more consistent trends out of our time series, we applied a smoothing function—a common practice when analyzing temporal data extracted from social media (O’Connor et al., 2010). Given a time-varying signal x(t), we apply a “boxcar” moving average over a window of the previous k days:

$${x}^{* }(t)=\frac{\mathop{\sum }\nolimits_{i = t-k}^{t}x(i)}{k};$$
(10)

We selected a window of one week (k = 7). Weekly time windows are typically used to smooth out both day-to-day variations as well as weekly periodicities (O’Connor et al., 2010). We applied the smoothing to all the time series: the language categories (\({z}_{c}^{* }(t)\)), the mentions of medical entities (\({f}_{e}^{* }(t)\)), the interaction types (\({f}_{i}^{* }(t)\)), and the foursquare visits (\({v}_{j}^{* }(t)\)).

Change-point detection

To identify phases characterized by different combinations of the language categories, we identified change-points—periods in which the values of all categories varied considerably at once. To quantify such variations, for each language category c, we computed \(\nabla ({z}_{c}^{* }(t))\), namely the daily average squared gradient (Lütkepohl, 2005) of the smoothed standardized fractions of that category. To calculate the gradient, we used the Python function numpy.gradient. The gradient provides a measure of the rate of increase or decrease of the signal; we consider the absolute value of the gradient, to account for the magnitude of change rather than the direction of change. To identify periods of consistent change as opposed to quick instantaneous shifts, we apply temporal smoothing (Eq. (10)) also to the time-series of gradients, and we denote the smoothed squared gradients with *. Last, we average the gradients of all language categories to obtain the overall gradient over time:

$$\nabla (t)=\frac{1}{D}\mathop{\sum }\limits_{d}{\nabla }^{* }({z}_{c}^{* }(t)).$$
(11)

Peaks in the time series (t) represent the days of highest variation, and we marked them as change-points. Using the Python function scipy.signal.find_peaks, we identified peaks as the local maxima whose values is higher than the average plus two standard deviations, as it is common practice (Palshikar, 2009).

Results

Language use until the first contagion wave

During the first wave, U.S. residents experienced what the pandemic entailed for the first time, and did so by going through an entire life-cycle, making this contagion wave self-contained: arrival of an unknown virus, skepticism, isolation measures, full lock-down, and first reopening. Figure 1A–C shows how the standardized fractions of all the language categories as per Formula (2) changed from 1 February to 15 April, the day in which restrictions in most states were lifted. The cell color encodes values higher than the average in red, and lower in blue. We partitioned the language categories according to the three psycho-social epidemics. Figure 1D shows the value of the average squared gradient over time (as per Formula (11)); peaks in the curve represent the days of high local variation. We marked the peaks above two standard deviations from the mean as change-points. We found two change-points that coincide with two key events: 27 February, the day of the announcement of the first infection in the country; and 24 March, the day of the announcement of the ‘stay at home’ orders. These change-points identify three phases, which are described next by dwelling on the peaks of the different language categories (days when their standardized fractions reached the maximum) and reporting the percentage increase at peak (the increase is compared to the average from 1 February to 15 April, and its peak is denoted by ‘max peak’ in Table 1). The first phase (refusal phase) was characterized by anxiety and fear. Death was frequently mentioned, with a peak on 11 February of +45% compared to its average during the whole time period. The pronoun they was used in this temporal state more than average; this suggests that the focus of discussion was on the implications of the viral epidemic on ‘others’, as this was when no infection had been discovered in the U.S. yet. All other language categories exhibited no significant variations, which reflected an overall situation of ‘business-as-usual.’

Fig. 1: The Epidemic Psychology on Twitter during the first contagion wave.
figure 1

AC evolution of the use of different language categories over time in tweets related to COVID-19. Each row in the heatmaps represents a language category (e.g., words expressing anxiety) that our manual coding associated with one of the three psycho-social epidemics. The cell color represents the daily standardized fraction of people who used words related to that category: values that are higher than the average are red and those that are lower are blue. Categories are partitioned in three groups according to the type of psycho-social epidemics they model: Fear, Morality, and Action. D Average gradient (i.e., instantaneous variation) of all the language categories; the peaks of gradient identify change-points—dates around which a considerable change in the use of multiple language categories happened at once. The dashed vertical lines that cross all the plots represent these change-points. EH temporal evolution of four families of indicators we used to corroborate the validity of the trends identified by the language categories. We checked internal validity by comparing the language categories with a custom keyword-search approach and two deep-learning NLP tools that extract types of social interactions and mentions of medical symptoms. We checked external validity by looking at mobility patterns in different venue categories as estimated by the GPS geo-localization service of the Foursquare mobile app. The timeline at the bottom of the figure marks some of the key events of the COVID-19 pandemic in the U.S. such as the announcements of the first infection of COVID-19 recorded.

The second phase (anger phase) began on 27 February with an outburst of negative emotions (predominantly anger), right after the first COVID-19 contagion in the U.S. was announced. The abstract fear of death was replaced by expressions of concrete health concerns, such as words expressing risk, and mentions of how body parts did feel. On 13 March, the federal government announced the state of national emergency, followed by the enforcement of state-level ‘stay at home’ orders. During those days, we observed a sharp increase of the use of the pronounIand of swear words (with a peak of +54% on 18 March), which hints at a climate of discussion characterized by conflict and polarization. At the same time, we observed an increase in the use of words related to the daily habits affected by the impending restriction policies, such as motion, social activities, and leisure. The mentions of words related to home peaked on 16 March (+38%), the day when the federal government announced social distancing guidelines to be in place for at least two weeks.

The third phase (acceptance phase) started on 24 March, the day after the first physical-distancing measures were imposed by law. The increased use of words of power and authority likely reflected the emergence of discussion around the new policies enforced by government officials and public agencies. As the death toll raised steadily—hitting the mark of 1000 deaths on 26 March—expressions of conflict faded away, and words of sadness became predominant. In those days of hardship, a sentiment of care for others and expressions of prosocial behavior became more frequent (+19% and +25%, respectively). Last, mentions of work-related activities peaked as many people either lost their job, or were compelled to work from home as result of the lockdown.

Thematic analysis

The language categories capture broad concepts related to Strong’s epidemic psychology theory, but they do not allow for an analysis of the fine-grained topics within each category. To study them, for each of the 87 combinations of language category and phase (29 language categories, for 3 phases), we listed the 100 most retweeted tweets (e.g., most popular tweets containing anxiety posted in the refusal phase). To identify overarching themes, we followed two steps that are commonly adopted in thematic analysis (Braun and Clarke, 2006; Smith and Shinebourne, 2012). We first applied open coding to identify key concepts that emerged across multiple tweets; specifically, one of the authors read all the tweets and marked them with keywords that reflected the key concepts expressed in the text. We then used axial coding to identify relationships between the most frequent keywords to summarize them in semantically cohesive themes. Themes were reviewed in a recursive manner rather than linear, by re-evaluating and adjusting them as new tweets were parsed. Table 2 summarizes the most recurring themes, together with some of their representative tweets. In the refusal phase, statements of skepticism were re-tweeted widely (Table 2, row 1). The epidemic was frequently depicted as a “foreign” problem (r. 2) and all activities kept business as usual (r. 3).

Table 2 Recurring themes in the three phases, found by the means of thematic analysis of tweets.

In the anger phase, the discussion was characterized by outrage against three main categories: foreigners—especially Chinese individuals, as Supplementary Materials detail—(r. 4), political opponents (r. 5), and people who adopted different behavioral responses to the outbreak (r. 6). This level of conflict corroborates Strong’s postulate of the “war against each other”. Science and religion were two prominent topics of discussion. A lively debate raged around the validity of scientists’ recommendations (r. 7). Some social groups put their hopes on God rather than on science (r. 8). Mentions of people self-isolating at home became very frequent, and highlighted the contrast between judicious individuals and careless crowds (r. 9).

Finally, during the acceptance phase, the outburst of anger gave in to the sorrow caused by the mourning of thousands of people (r. 10). By accepting the real threat of the virus, our Twitter users were more open to find collective solutions to the problem and overcome fear with hope (r. 11). Although the positive attitude towards the authorities seemed prevalent, some users expressed disappointment against the restrictions imposed (r. 12). Those who were isolated at home started imagining a life beyond the isolation, especially in relation to reopening businesses (r. 13).

Comparison with other behavioral markers

To assess the validity of our approach, we compared the previous results with the output of alternative text-mining techniques applied to the same data (internal validity), and with real-world mobility traces (external validity).

Comparison with other text mining techniques

We processed the very same social media posts with three alternative text-mining techniques (Fig. 1E–G). In Table 3, we reported the three language categories with the strongest correlations with each behavioral marker.

Table 3 (Left) Correlation of our language categories with behavioral markers computed with alternative techniques and datasets.

First, to allow for interpretable and explainable results, we applied a simple word-matching method that relies on a custom lexicon containing three categories of words reflecting consumption of alcohol, physical exercising, and economic concerns, as those aspects have been found to characterize the COVID-19 pandemic (The Economist, 2020). We measured the daily fraction of users mentioning words in each of those categories (Fig. 1E). In the refusal phase, the frequency of any of these words did not significantly increase. In the anger phase, the frequency of words related to economy peaked, and that related to alcohol consumption peaked shortly after that. Table 3 shows that economy-related words were highly correlated with the use of anxiety words (r = 0.73), which is in line with studies indicating that the degree of apprehension for the declining economy was comparable to that of health-hazard concerns (Bareket-Bojmel et al., 2020; Fetzer et al., 2020). Words of alcohol consumption were most correlated with the language dimensions of body (r = 0.70), feel (r = 0.62), home (r = 0.58); in the period were health concerns were at their peak, home isolation caused a rising tide of alcohol use (Da et al., 2020; Finlay and Gilmore, 2020). Finally, in the acceptance phase, the frequency of words related to physical exercise was significant; this happened at the same time when the use of positive words expressing togetherness was at its highest—affiliation (r = 0.95), posemo (r = 0.93), we (r = 0.92). All these results match our previous interpretations of the peaks for our language categories.

Second, since it is unclear whether a simple word count approach is effective in studying how the three psycho-social epidemics unfolded over time, we additionally applied the deep-learning approach that extracts mentions of expressions of conflict, social support, and power. Figure 1F shows the min-max normalized scores of the fraction of users posting tweets labeled with each of these three interaction types (as per Formula (5)). In the refusal phase, conflict increased—this is when anxiety and blaming foreigners were recurring themes in Twitter. In the anger phase, conflict peaked (similar to anxiety words, r = 0.88), yet, since the first lock-down measures were announced, initial expressions of power and of social support gradually increased as well. Finally, in the acceptance phase, social support peaked. Support was most correlated with the categories of affiliation (r = 0.98), positive emotions (r = 0.96), and we (r = 0.94) (Table 3); power was most correlated with prosocial (r = 0.95), care (r = 0.94), and authority (r = 0.94). Again, our previous interpretations concerning the existence of a phase of conflict followed by a phase of social support were further confirmed by the deep-learning tool, which, as opposed to our dictionary-based approaches, does not rely on word matching.

Third, we used the deep-learning tool that extracts mentions of medical entities from text (Scepanovic et al., 2020). Out of all the entities extracted, we focused on the 100 most frequently mentioned and grouped them into two families of symptoms, respectively, those related to physical health (e.g., “fever”, “cough”, “sick”) and those related to mental health (e.g., “depression”, “stress”) (Brooks et al., 2020). The min−max normalized fractions of users posting tweets containing mentions of these symptoms (as per Formula (7)) are shown in Fig. 1G. In refusal phase, the frequency of symptom mentions did not change. In the anger phase, instead, physical symptoms started to be mentioned, and they were correlated with the language categories expressing panic and physical health concerns—swear (r = 0.83), feel (r = 0.77), and negate (r = 0.67). In the acceptance phase, mentions of mental symptoms became most frequent. Interestingly, mental symptoms peaked when the Twitter discourse was characterized by positive feelings and prosocial interactions—affiliation (r = 0.91), we (r = 0.88), and posemo (r = 0.85); this is in line with recent studies that found that the psychological toll of COVID-19 has similar traits to post-traumatic stress disorders and its symptoms might lag several weeks from the period of initial panic and forced isolation (Dutheil et al., 2020; Galea et al., 2020; Liang et al., 2020).

Comparison with mobility traces

To test for the external validity of our language categories, we compared their temporal trends with the mobility data from Foursquare. We picked three venue categories: Grocery shops, Travel & Transport, and Outdoors & Recreation to reflect three different types of fundamental human needs (Maslow, 1943): the primary need of getting food supplies, the secondary need of moving around freely (or to limit mobility for safety), and the higher-level need of being entertained. In Fig. 1H, we show the min-max normalized number of visits over time (as per Formula (9)). The periods of higher variations of the normalized number of visits match the transitions between the three phases. In the refusal phase, mobility patterns did not change. In the anger phase, instead, travel started to drop, and grocery shopping peaked, supporting the interpretation of a phase characterized by a wave of panic-induced stockpiling and a compulsion to save oneself—it co-occurred with the peak of use of the pronoun I (r = 0.80)—rather than helping others. Finally, in the acceptance phase, the panic around grocery shopping faded away, and the number of visits to parks and outdoor spaces increased.

Embedding epidemic psychology in real-time models

To embed our operationalization of epidemic psychology into real-time models (e.g., epidemiological models, urban mobility models), our measures need to work at any point in time during a new pandemic; yet, given their current definitions, they do not: that is because they are normalized values over the whole period of study (Fig. 1A–C). To fix that, we designed a new composite measure that does not rely on full temporal knowledge, and a corresponding detection method that determines which of the three phases one is in at any given point in time.

First, for each language category c, we computed the average value of fc (as per Formula (1)) during the first day of the epidemic, specifically μ[0, 1](fc). During the first day, 86k users tweeted. We experimented with longer periods (up to a week and 0.4M users), and obtained qualitatively similar results. We used the averages computed on this initial period as reference values for later measurements. The assumption behind this approach is that the modeler would know the set of relevant hashtags in the initial stages of the pandemic, which is reasonable considering that this was the case for all the major pandemics occurred in the last decade (Chew and Eysenbach, 2010; Fu et al., 2016; Oyeyemi et al., 2014). Starting from the second day, we then calculated the percent change of the fc values compared to the reference values:

$${{\Delta }}{ \% }_{c}(t)=\frac{{f}_{c}(t)-{\mu }_{[0,1]}({f}_{c})}{{\mu }_{[0,1]}({f}_{c})}.$$
(12)

For each phase, we defined a parsimonious measure composed only of two dimensions: the dimension most positively associated with the phase (expressed in percent change) minus that most negatively associated with it (e.g., (death-I) for the refusal phase). To identify such dimensions, we trained three logistic regression binary classifiers (one per phase). For each phase, we marked with label 1 all the days that were included in that phase and with 0 those that were not. Then, we trained a classifier to estimate the probability \({P}_{\mathrm {{phas{e}}}_{i}}(t)\) that day t belongs to phase i, out of the Δ%c(t) values for all categories. During training, the logistic regression learned coefficients for each of the categories. On average, the classifiers were able to identify the correct phase for 98% of the days.

The regressions coefficients were then used to rank the language category by their predictive power. Table 4 shows the top three positive beta coefficients and bottom three negative ones for each of the three phases. The top and bottom categories of all phases belong to the LIWC lexicon. For each phase, we subtracted the top category from the bottom category without considering their beta coefficients, as these would require, again, full temporal knowledge:

$$\begin{array}{ll}&{{\Delta }}{ \% }_{Refusal}={{\Delta }}{ \% }_{death}-{{\Delta }}{ \% }_{I}\\ &{{\Delta }}{ \% }_{Anger}={{\Delta }}{ \% }_{swear}-{{\Delta }}{ \% }_{death}\\ &{{\Delta }}{ \% }_{Acceptance}={{\Delta }}{ \% }_{sad}-{{\Delta }}{ \% }_{anxiety}\end{array}$$
(13)

The resulting composite measure has the same change-points (Fig. 3A) as the full-knowledge measure’s (Fig. 1), suggesting that the real-time and parsimonious computation does not compromise the original trends. In a real-time scenario, transition between phases are captured by the changes of the dominant measure; for example, when the refusal curve is overtaken by the anger curve. In addition, we correlated the composite measures with each of the behavioral markers we used for validation (Fig. 1E–H) to find which are the markers most typically associated with each of the phases. We reported the correlations in Table 3. During the refusal phase, conflictual interactions were frequent (r = 0.58) and long-range mobility was common (r = 0.62); during the anger phase, as mobility reduced (Engle et al., 2020; Gao et al., 2020), some people hoarded groceries and alcohol (Da et al., 2020; Finlay and Gilmore, 2020) and expressed concerns for their physical health (r = 0.81) and for the economy (Bareket-Bojmel et al., 2020; Fetzer et al., 2020); last, during the acceptance phase, some people ventured outdoors, started exercising more, and expressed a stronger will to support each other (r = 0.90), in the wake of a rising tide of deaths and mental health symptoms (r = 0.85) (Dutheil et al., 2020; Galea et al., 2020; Liang et al., 2020).

Table 4 Top three positive and bottom negative beta coefficients of the logistic regression models for the three phases.

Language use after the first contagion wave

After the first wave and the “stay-at-home” lock-down at the end of March, for the remaining part of the year, there were two other contagion waves (Fig. 2A): one at the beginning of June, and the other at the beginning of October. In a way similar to the first wave, these two others were both associated with significant changes in the use of language (Fig. 2B): we had two change peaks for the second wave (i.e., 27 May and 9 June), and one for the third (2 October). This comes at no surprise as these two periods corresponded to two widely discussed events: the first 100K deaths in the U.S., and President Donald Trump testing positive for COVID-19. In particular, Fig. 3B shows that these changes were both to do with the rumping up of the categories associated with the anger phase: discussions of changes in mobility, and posts characterized by anger and, more generally, by negative emotions were predominant. By contrast, the categories associated with refusal and acceptance diverged from each other: unsurprisingly, throughout the year, refusal gradually died off, while acceptance increasingly took hold. Overall, we observed two classes of pattern (Fig. 3B). First, the three phases were not always orthogonal but blended together at times. During the second contagion wave, for example, anger and acceptance were both predominant, and had been so for several months. Second, the use of language had a cyclical nature. During each contagion wave, three consecutive local peaks (local maxima) were observed: refusal first, then anger, finally acceptance. This can be observed for all the three contagion waves. Such a cyclical nature was also reflected in our behavioral markers (Fig. 4E–H). For each wave, mentions of conflict peaked to then be followed by mentions of support and power (Fig. 4F). The same went for medical conditions: mentions of physical health peaked to then be followed by mentions of mental health (Fig. 4G).

Fig. 2: Variation of the language categories over time.
figure 2

The number of recorded infections in the U.S. (A), and the average gradient (i.e., instantaneous variation) of all the language categories (B). There were three contagion waves in the year (shaded areas in A) and, for each wave, there were peaks in the gradient (marked with circles in B), which identify change-points, that is, periods in which the use of language considerably changed.

Fig. 3: Aggregated measures of the three phases of Epidemic Psychology over time.
figure 3

The evolution of the language categories associated with refusal, of those associated with anger, and of those associated with acceptance, zooming out from the first contagion wave (A) to the three waves during the entire 2020 (B).

Fig. 4: The Epidemic Psychology on Twitter during the three contagion waves (shown in the top most panel) during the entire year of 2020.
figure 4

AH Temporally expands AH in Fig. 1 with the only difference that the heatmaps in AC here show values that were standardized using the mean and standard deviation calculated on the whole period of study, from 1 February to 31 December.

Discussion

Findings beyond Strong’s model

Strong’s theory offers a framework upon which to operationalize the three psycho-social epidemics from the use of language but does not specifically describe how these epidemics unfold. Based on our data-driven results, we confirmed that the three epidemics are indeed present in our social conversations spanning almost one year, and they unfolded in ways that allowed us to enrich what Strong initially hypothesized in relation to four main aspects.

First, Strong’s theory predicts the presence of the three psycho-social epidemics but does not describe how they would be related to each other over time. We found that the three epidemics, as expressed by the relative presence of their relevant language categories, simultaneously raised and fell over time, and did so in relation to one another, demarcating three specific temporal phases. Over time, we identified three specific combinations of the epidemics that generated three phases our Twitter users went through: an initial refusal phase, an anger phase, and a final acceptance phase. Since these temporal phases partly resemble the stages of grief by Kübler-Ross et al. (1972), a promising direction for future work is to explore the relationship between these phases and the stages of grief.

Second, Strong’s narration slightly hints at a typical sequence of events according to which the epidemic of fear activates before that of morality, which is then followed by that of action. We indeed observed a similar set of events yet these events were not strictly sequential but rather cyclical. Interestingly, every new cycle started in conjunction with the same specific event: the diffusion rate of the virus reaching a local maximum. Shortly after every sharp increase in the diffusion rate, a cycle of refusal, anger, and acceptance unfolded among our Twitter users.

Third, Strong’s framework does not make any explicit distinction between the initial stages of an epidemic and its final stages. We found two regimes that considerably separated the initial stages from the later stages: in the initial cycle, the variation of the three epidemics was of a larger magnitude than those in the two subsequent cycles.

Last, Strong’s theory should be used as a descriptive framework and, as such, cannot be used to explore short-term variations. By contrast, a data-driven experimental work such as ours is able to point out when such variations potentially took place, as Fig. 4 shows. In future work, these points of change could be subject to qualitative inquiry, which might well enrich the original formulation of the theory.

Implications

New infectious diseases break out abruptly, and public health agencies try to rely on detailed planning yet often find themselves to improvise around their playbook. They are constantly confronting not only the health epidemic but also the three psycho-social epidemics. Measuring the effects of epidemics on societal dynamics and population mental health has been an open research problem for a long time, and multidisciplinary approaches have been called for (Holmes et al., 2020). We contributed to this line of research by operationalizing Strong’s model and successfully testing it on Twitter in the U.S. Since our methodology can be applied to any textual data, future work may well study alternative (even cross-cultural) population segments. Since our language categories are not tailored to a specific epidemic (e.g., they do not reflect any specific symptom an epidemic is associated with), our approach can be applied to a future epidemic, provided that the set of relevant hashtags associated with the epidemic is known; this is a reasonable assumption to make though, considering that the consensus on Twitter hashtags is reached quickly (Baronchelli, 2018), and that several epidemics that occurred in the last decade sparked discussions on Twitter since their early days (Chew and Eysenbach, 2010; Fu et al., 2016; Oyeyemi et al., 2014).

Our method complements the numerous cross-sectional studies on the psychological impact of health epidemics conducted on representative population samples (Brooks et al., 2020; Shultz et al., 2015), not least because it collects real-time statistics of implicit behavioral signals, which are orthogonal to survey responses.

For computer science researchers, our method could provide a starting point for developing more sophisticated tools for monitoring psycho-social epidemics. Furthermore, from the theoretical standpoint, our work provides the first operationalization of Strong’s model of the epidemic psychology, and widens its theoretical implications by observing cyclical phases of diffusion of the psycho-social epidemics.

Finally, the ability to systematically characterize the three psycho-social epidemics from the use of language on social media makes it possible to embed epidemic psychology into models currently used to tackle epidemics such as mobility models (Bansal et al., 2016). To see how, consider that, in digital epidemiology (Bauch and Galvani, 2013; Salathe et al., 2012), some parameters of epidemic models are initialized or adjusted based on a variety of digital data to account for co-determinants of the spreading process that are hard to quantify with traditional data sources, especially in the first stages of the outbreak. This is particularly useful when modeling social and psychological processes such as risk perception (Bagnoli et al., 2007; Moinet et al., 2018). Interestingly, these approaches are designed to deal with partial data, therefore they can benefit even from digital data that is incomplete and—like in the case of our Twitter-based study—not necessarily representative of the whole population (Salathe et al., 2012).

Limitations

Future work could improve our work in five main aspects. First, we focused only on one viral epidemic, without being able to compare it to others. Yet, if one were to obtain past social media data during the outbreaks of diseases like Zika (Fu et al., 2016), Ebola (Oyeyemi et al., 2014), and the H1N1 influenza (Chew and Eysenbach, 2010), one could apply our methodology in those contexts as well, and identify similarities and differences. For example, one could study how mortality rates or speed of spreading influence the representation of Strong’s epidemic psychology on social media.

Second, our geographical focus was the entire United States and, as such, was coarse and limited in scope. In Supplementary Materials, we broke-down the analysis of temporal phases for individual states in the U.S., and observed no substantial differences across states. In the future, one could conduct a more systematic analysis on a finer geographical granularity, relate differences between states to known events (e.g., a governor’s decisions, prevalence of cases, media landscape, and residents’ cultural traits). In particular, recent studies suggested that the public reaction to COVID-19 varied across the U.S. states depending on their political leaning (Grossman et al., 2020; Painter and Qiu, 2020). One could also apply our methodology to other English-speaking countries, to investigate how cultural dimensions (Hofstede et al., 2005) and cross-cultural personality trait variations (Bleidorn et al., 2013) might influence the three psycho-social epidemics.

Third, the three psycho-social epidemics were not always orthogonal to each other but did blend together at times. Future work could focus on those particular periods of time to determine whether the use either of finer-grained categorizations of language or of event detection techniques other than our change-point detection (Aiello et al., 2013) could disentangle those periods in theoretically meaningful ways.

Fourth, our study is limited to Twitter, mainly because Twitter is the largest open stream of real-time social media data. The practice of using Twitter as a way of modeling the psychological state of a country carries its own limitations. Despite having a rather high penetration in the U.S. (around 20% of adults, according to the latest estimates (Perrin and Anderson, 2018)), its user base is not representative of the general population (Li et al., 2013). Additionally, Twitter is notoriously populated by bots (Ferrara et al., 2016; Varol et al., 2017), automated accounts that are often used to amplify specific topics or view points. Bots played an important role to steer the discussion on several events of broad public interest (Bessi and Ferrara, 2016; Broniatowski et al., 2018), and it is reasonable to expect that they have a role in COVID-related discussions too, as some studies seem to suggest (Yang et al., 2020). To partly discount their impact, since they tend to have anomalous levels of activity (especially retweeting (Bessi and Ferrara, 2016)), we performed two tests. First, we computed all our measures at user-level rather than tweet-level, which counter anomalous levels of activity. Second, we replicated our temporal analysis excluding retweets, and obtained very similar results. In the future, one could attempt to adapt our framework to different sources of online data, for example to web search queries—which have proven useful to identify different phases of the public reactions to the COVID-19 pandemic (Husnayain et al., 2020).

Last, as Strong himself acknowledged in his seminal paper: “any sharp separation between different types of epidemic psychology is a dubious business.” Our work has operationalized each psycho-social epidemic independently. In the future, modeling the relationships among the three epidemics might identify hitherto hidden emergent properties.