Next Article in Journal
Use of the Dietary Inflammatory Index to Assess the Diet of Young Physically Active Men
Next Article in Special Issue
The Mental Health Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic Second Wave on Shielders and Their Family Members
Previous Article in Journal
Evaluating the Effect of Chinese Environmental Regulation on Corporate Sustainability Performance: The Mediating Role of Green Technology Innovation
Previous Article in Special Issue
Perceived Risk of COVID-19 and Employee Decision-Making: How Psychological Distress during the Pandemic Increases Negative Performance Outcomes among Healthcare Workers
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

COVID-19 Vaccine Misperceptions in a Community Sample of Adults Aged 18–49 Years in Australia

1
Faculty of Medicine and Health, Sydney Health Literacy Lab, School of Public Health, The University of Sydney, Sydney 2006, Australia
2
Population Wellbeing and Environment Research Lab, School of Health and Society, University of Wollongong, Wollongong 2522, Australia
3
Western Sydney Diabetes, Western Sydney Local Health District, Sydney 2006, Australia
4
National Centre for Immunisation Research and Surveillance, Kids Research, Sydney Children’s Hospital Network, Sydney 2006, Australia
5
School of Population Health, Faculty of Medicine and Health, The University of New South Wales, Sydney 2006, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19(11), 6883; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116883
Submission received: 3 May 2022 / Revised: 30 May 2022 / Accepted: 30 May 2022 / Published: 4 June 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue The Role of Behavioural Science during COVID-19)

Abstract

:
Central to a successful population vaccination program is high uptake of vaccines. However, COVID-19 vaccine uptake may be impeded by beliefs based on misinformation. We sought to understand the prevalence and nature of misbeliefs about COVID-19 vaccines, and identify associated factors, shortly after commencement of Australia’s national vaccine rollout. A cross-sectional survey was administered to unvaccinated young adults (n = 2050) in Australia aged 18–49 years (mean age 33 years), 13 July–21 August 2021. This sample was previously under-represented in COVID-19 research but shown to have less willingness to vaccinate. Two thirds of participants agreed with at least one misbelief item. Misperceptions about COVID-19 vaccines were found to be significantly associated with lower health literacy, less knowledge about vaccines, lower perceived personal risk of COVID-19, greater endorsement of conspiracy beliefs, and lower confidence and trust in government and scientific institutions. Misbeliefs were more common in participants with less educational attainment, in younger age groups, and in males, as per previous research. Understanding determinants and barriers to vaccination uptake, such as knowledge and beliefs based on misinformation, can help to shape effective public health communication and inform debunking efforts at this critical time and in the future.

1. Introduction

The COVID-19 pandemic has prompted the largest vaccination program in history [1]. Central to a successful population vaccination program is high uptake of vaccines. Longitudinal surveys of adults in Australia conducted prior to the availability of COVID-19 vaccines and when case numbers of the virus were low (April–July 2020) showed high rates of vaccination willingness (86–90% willing) [2,3]. However, younger adults perceived themselves to be at lower risk of infection and were less willing to receive a vaccine [4]. This has also been shown internationally [5]. Common concerns about COVID-19 vaccines include vaccine safety, side effects, and long-term effects [3,6,7]. Access barriers to vaccination such as cost and local availability have also been identified as influencing COVID-19 vaccine uptake internationally [8] and vaccination willingness among younger adults [9].
Misinformation about vaccines has been shown to negatively influence people’s attitudes and intentions towards vaccination, such as in the context of human papillomavirus (HPV) and measles-mumps-rubella (MMR) vaccines [10]. Several studies now suggest that COVID-19 vaccine uptake may be impeded by beliefs based on misinformation [11,12,13]. For example, Loomba et al. reported a 6.2–6.4% point decline in vaccination intention among people in the UK and US following exposure to misinformation [11]. It has also been shown globally that certain sociodemographic groups have a greater tendency to believe misinformation (e.g., [14]). In Australia, 13–17% of 4362 adult respondents to a national survey indicated agreement with various forms of misinformation about COVID-19 in 2020 [15]. Lower trust in institutions and greater rejection of official accounts were associated with stronger misbeliefs. Another key finding of this study was that agreement with misinformation was significantly associated with younger age. This survey was conducted many months before COVID-19 vaccines were approved in Australia and large-scale COVID outbreaks (Delta and Omicron) that have since occurred nationally.
At the time of the initial vaccination rollout in Australia, little was known or had been published on public knowledge and beliefs about COVID-19 vaccines. Some prior research demonstrated an association between vaccine hesitancy or refusal and having inadequate knowledge about COVID-19 [2,6]. As these studies were conducted prior to widespread vaccine rollout, vaccine intentions were hypothetical, and knowledge measures were based on knowledge of recommended actions to protect oneself from contracting COVID-19 rather than vaccine-specific knowledge. The current study used a validated measure of vaccine knowledge to determine whether people’s knowledge of vaccines was associated with the likelihood of them holding misbeliefs about COVID vaccines.
In this study, we sought to understand the prevalence and nature of beliefs about vaccines generally and COVID-19 vaccines specifically, and factors associated with vaccine misbeliefs, shortly after the commencement of the Australian national vaccine rollout. Our target population comprised unvaccinated younger adults (18–49 years), as this sample was under-represented in COVID-19 research but shown to have less willingness to vaccinate [6]. Understanding determinants of and barriers to vaccination uptake, such as knowledge and beliefs based on misinformation, can help to shape effective public health communication and inform debunking efforts at this critical time and in the future.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Design

A national cross-sectional online survey built and administered using the Qualtrics (SAP SE) online survey platform. The study was approved by the University of Sydney Human Research Ethics Committee (2020/212).

2.2. Setting

The survey was distributed nationally with data collected between 13 July to 21 August 2021. The highly infectious SARS-CoV-2 Delta variant (B.1.617.2) of COVID-19 was first detected in Australia in June 2021. In response to this outbreak, almost half of Australia’s population and most major cities were not permitted under public health law to leave their place of residence (with exemptions) from early July 2021, and new record daily cases in Australia were recorded in August 2021 [16]. The states of New South Wales and Victoria were disproportionately impacted by local outbreaks [17]. Until this point, COVID-19 had been relatively well controlled by Australia’s strong public health measures, so this was a new experience of risk exposure for the Australian community.
Australia’s nationwide COVID-19 vaccination program began on 21 February 2021. At the time of the survey, 10.2–19.2% (13 July–21 August) of the adult population aged 16–49 years were fully vaccinated against COVID-19 (i.e., had received two doses of vaccine), and 15.5–34.4% (13 July–21 August) over 16 years had received at least one dose [18]. We have reported elsewhere that in July–August 2021, 43% of adults 18–49 years intended to receive a vaccine as soon as possible, with 6% responding that they would never get a COVID-19 vaccine [9].

2.3. Participants

Participants were aged 18 to 49 years, able to read and understand English, currently residing in Australia, and had not yet received a COVID-19 vaccine. Individuals who had received any dose of a COVID-19 vaccine were excluded. We recruited participants through Taverner Research, an Australian-owned market research company, using an online panel with approximately 1 million panel members aged under 50 years. Panel members were sent an email invitation to participate in the study and received points for completing the survey, which they could redeem for a range of rewards including gift cards, cash, and vouchers.

2.4. Measures

Relevant measures for this analysis are detailed in Table 1. Sociodemographic variables collected included age, education, gender, and residential state. Health literacy was assessed using a single item literacy screener [19]. Digital health literacy was assessed using the eHealth Literacy Scale [20].
For the main outcome measure—COVID-19 vaccine related misperceptions, a list of items was curated from international official health myth buster websites [21,22,23], published literature and mainstream media and commonly reported myths.
We also measured general vaccine knowledge using a validated scale [24], acceptance of vaccine conspiracies, perceived risk of COVID-19, and confidence and trust in government and institutions.
Participants were asked whether they had encountered any negative information about COVID-19 vaccines, and if so, where. They were also asked to briefly describe any content discouraging people from vaccinating that they had come across in the past week.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

Analyses were conducted using Stata/IC (v16.1; StataCorp LLC, College Station, TX, USA). The threshold for statistical significance was set at p < 0.05 (2-tailed). Descriptive statistics (means and SD for continuous variables, and frequency and relative frequency for categorical variables) were calculated for participant characteristics and study outcomes.
Multivariable truncated linear regression analyses of mean vaccine misbeliefs were conducted, controlling for age, gender, educational attainment, and state of residence. Truncation was based on possible values of the derived outcome variable (i.e., lower bound of 1 and an upper bound of 5). A base model was first constructed including only the control variables listed above, and then, as a full model with all potential explanatory variables included (see Table 2). p-values less than 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Estimates are provided to 3 significant figures.
Responses to all misbelief items at baseline were moderately correlated (pairwise Rs between 0.21–0.64), with good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.9).
Qualitative data (free text responses in the survey) were analysed using content analysis [31]. BC and MT read through all responses to familiarize themselves with the content and generated a preliminary list of recurring themes. These were discussed with RD, TC and KP. BC and MT then finalized the coding framework and applied it to the entire dataset. Discrepancies were discussed and resolved at team meetings. We assessed level of agreement between the reviewers using the Cohen kappa statistic, which indicated high agreement (k = 0.968). Descriptive statistics were calculated to summarise the frequency of occurrence of each code. Illustrative quotes are presented in the results to highlight each theme.

3. Results

Sample Characteristics

Characteristics of the 2050 participants are summarised in Table 2. Mean age of participants was 33 years, 42.3% (n = 868) held a university degree, 83.3% (n = 1707) were classified as having adequate health literacy, and most of the sample resided in New South Wales (30.2%; n = 619), Victoria (25%; n = 513), or Queensland (21.4%; n = 439) (Australia’s three most populous states). Our sample had slightly higher educational attainment when compared to national data (42.3% versus 37.8% university degree) but was otherwise comparable to national estimates [32].

4. Confidence and Trust in Vaccines

The majority of participants had high trust in the safety (87.9%) and effectiveness (85.8%) of childhood vaccines and in flu (safety 79.2%; effectiveness 75%) and travel vaccines (safety 73.7%; effectiveness 73.5%) (Table 3). Two-thirds (~65%) of participants trusted the safety and effectiveness of the Pfizer vaccine, while a lower proportion (~40%) indicated the same trust for the AstraZeneca vaccine.
Regarding confidence in COVID-19 vaccine benefits, 70.5% thought that getting a COVID-19 vaccine will protect other people in their community, 66.3% said that a COVID-19 vaccine will be moderately or very safe, 62.5% of participants indicated that getting a COVID-19 vaccine will be important for their health, and 47.6% were moderately or very concerned that a COVID-19 vaccine would cause them to have a serious reaction.

5. Misperceptions, Conspiracy Beliefs, and Vaccine Knowledge

Table 4 shows mean scores for the COVID-19 vaccine misbelief and acceptance of vaccine conspiracy items, and percentage correct for the vaccine knowledge items.

5.1. COVID-19 Vaccine Misperceptions

Overall, 79.4% (n = 1628) of participants had a mean score of less than 3/5 on the vaccine misbelief items, indicating an average disagreement with the vaccine misbelief items, and 10.6% (n = 218) strongly disagreed with all items. Sixty-six percent (n = 1352) of participants scored higher than 3 on at least one item, indicating at least one misbelief.
Table 5 shows the most common COVID-19 vaccine misperceptions were: COVID vaccinated people can affect non-vaccinated people’s health (agreed: n = 602, 29.4%), people who have had a COVID-19 vaccine shed the virus to others (agreed: n = 459, 22.4%), and COVID-19 vaccines cause immune damage (agreed: n = 386, 18.8%). Seven hundred twenty-one (35.2%) participants were unsure regarding whether COVID-19 vaccines have been linked to infertility.
General anti-vaccination beliefs were held by some participants; 12.6% (n = 258) of the sample indicated “true” that “vaccines cause autism”, with 61.9% (n = 1269) stating that this statement is false. A 25.5% share of participants (n = 523) were unsure.

5.2. Vaccine-Related Conspiracy Beliefs

As shown in Table 6, 651 (31.8%) participants agreed with the statement “people are deceived about the effectiveness of vaccines”. Additionally, 607 (29.6%) agreed that people are deceived about vaccine safety, and 571 (27.9%) agreed that drug companies cover up the dangers of vaccines.

5.3. Vaccine Knowledge

On average, participants scored correctly on 49% (~4 of the 8 vaccine knowledge questions). As shown in Table 7, most participants scored correctly on the following three items: “the effectiveness of vaccines has been proven” (Agree: n = 1629; 79.5%), “vaccines are not needed because diseases can be treated” (Disagree: n = 1619; 79.0%), and “without broadly applied vaccine programs, smallpox would still exist” (Agree: n = 1507; 73.5%).
Participants were more likely to have incorrect knowledge about vaccine safety (“the doses of chemicals used in vaccines are not dangerous for humans” (Agree: n = 504; 24.6%)), vaccines and the immune system (“the immune system is overloaded if we are given too many vaccinations” (Disagree: n = 473; 23.1%)), and disease resistance (“people would be more resistant to diseases if they were not given so many vaccines” (Disagree: n = 384; 18.7%)).
Regarding uncertain responses, 946 (46.1%) of participants did not know whether vaccinations increase the occurrence of allergies. There was also uncertainty in response to questions about vaccine safety and side effects.

5.4. Factors Associated with Greater Agreement with COVID-19 Vaccine Misbeliefs

Estimated marginal mean values from the multivariable regression model of COVID-19 vaccine misbeliefs are provided in Table 8. Greater agreement with COVID-19 vaccine misbeliefs was significantly associated with younger age (p < 0.01), male gender (p < 0.01), and lower educational attainment (p < 0.001).
After controlling for age, gender, education, and state of residence, misperceptions about COVID-19 vaccines were found to be significantly associated with lower health literacy, less knowledge about vaccines, lower perceived personal risk of COVID-19, greater endorsement of non-COVID conspiracy beliefs, lower confidence in government, and lower trust in scientific institutions.
The only control variable that remained significantly associated with COVID-19 vaccine misperceptions in the adjusted model was lower education, while age and gender became non-significant. There were no differences observed between states in any model.

5.5. Content Discouraging People from Having a COVID-19 Vaccine

A total of 963 participants (47% of sample) responded to the free-text question: “In the last week have you come across any content discouraging people from vaccinating? (i.e., on your social media, from friends, at the workplace)?”. Seventeen themes were developed which captured the type of discouraging content participants witnessed (Table S1). The three most reported themes are summarised below, and the top 5 themes are shown in Table 9, with example quotes.
  • Negative vaccine side effects and vaccine risk (n = 384, 39.9%): Included responses pertaining to concerns about specific and general side effects of the COVID-19 vaccine as well as concerns about adverse events post-vaccination and overall safety of the vaccine. Specific side effects of concern included blood clots, fertility and reproductive concerns, motor, cognitive and neurological disorders, AstraZeneca-specific concerns, shaking, paralysis, pain, fever and fainting.
  • Conspiracy theories (n = 133, 13.8%): Example conspiracy theories commonly raised included virus legitimacy scepticism, 5G, tracking, microchips, magnetism caused by the vaccine and vaccine causing COVID-19 or other disease.
  • Antivax or hesitance towards vaccine (n = 115, 11.9%): Responses included general anti-vaccination sentiments (e.g., “strongly against vaccine for well-informed reasons” or “not liking vaccines”), reluctance to receive vaccinations and mentions of “Antivaxxers” as community.
Of the 963 participants, 268 (27.8%) also reported the source of the content discouraging vaccination. From these, the majority (n = 182; 67.9%) reported the source to be social media, with commonly reported platforms including Facebook, Instagram and TikTok. Family, friends and colleagues were the next most frequently mentioned source (n = 57; 21.3%), followed by traditional media (n = 46; 17.2%) (Table S2).

6. Discussion

This analysis showed that one in five younger adults aged 18–49 years in Australia on average agreed with some items of misinformation about COVID-19 vaccines, with two thirds indicating agreement with at least one misbelief item. Misperceptions about COVID-19 vaccines were found to be significantly associated with lower health literacy, less knowledge about vaccines, lower perceived personal risk of COVID-19, greater endorsement of non-COVID conspiracy beliefs, lower confidence in government, and lower trust in scientific institutions. Misbeliefs were more common in participants with less educational attainment, in younger age groups, and in males. These findings are similar to those of our earlier COVID misinformation study [15]; however, it comprised an older age cohort (age range 18–90 years; mean age 43 years) versus the younger sample of the current study (age range 18–49 years; mean age 33 years). The results also replicate international studies that have reported on predictors of susceptibility to misinformation (e.g., [14]); however, these studies did not examine predictors of vaccine-specific misinformation.
The most common COVID-19 vaccine misperceptions in this study were perceptions that COVID vaccinated people can affect non-vaccinated people’s health (refer to footnote in Table 5), shed the virus to others, and that COVID-19 vaccines cause immune damage. Uncertainty was also notably high regarding whether COVID-19 vaccines have been linked to infertility. Isolating specific misbeliefs about COVID-19 vaccines can support communication experts and public health agencies to better target their messages to address vaccine misunderstandings. Such misperceptions are relatively common in the broader vaccine context; for example, 21% of Australian parents surveyed in 2017 believed that vaccines can cause autism [27].
While around 20% of our sample on average agreed or strongly agreed with misbelief items, this is of concern, given that increased susceptibility to misbeliefs has been found to reduce self-reported compliance with COVID prevention measures including intention to get vaccinated, likelihood of recommending COVID-19 vaccines to family and friends [11,14], and confidence in the safety of COVID-19 vaccines [12]. Uptake of COVID-19 booster injections (i.e., additional vaccine doses after primary vaccinations) has been low relative to initially high vaccine uptake in Australia [33]. It will be imperative to prioritise correcting misperceptions about COVID-19 vaccines such as those identified in this study to achieve higher rates of booster injections. It is also important to recognise that continued success in COVID-19 and other vaccination programs relies on general community support, which may be harder to gain if one in five people hold misbeliefs about COVID-19 vaccines.
Vaccine misperceptions were more common in those with lower health literacy and educational attainment. These groups have also been shown to be less willing to receive a COVID vaccine [34]. Understanding and critically evaluating health information is a fundamental challenge for a large proportion of the community [35,36], and especially in the context of the COVID-19 infodemic [37,38]. Our study helps to pinpoint specific beliefs about vaccines that can be addressed in health literate-sensitive formats as a starting point, with an overall aim to improve health literacy of populations in need of information about COVID vaccines and in those providing that information.
Participant scores on the vaccine knowledge scale were relatively low but similar to other studies that have used this validated measure (e.g., [39]). This warrants further investigation in younger populations given that those with lower knowledge of vaccines generally showed more misbeliefs about COVID-19 vaccines, and have demonstrated less likelihood of vaccination [39]. Our results highlight specific gaps in our communications about vaccines generally, and specifically about safe vaccine doses, immune system overload, and vaccine resistance. Targeting vaccine literacy may be beneficial in countering vaccine misperceptions [40]. However, vaccine beliefs are complex, and people may have misperceptions about vaccines for a variety of reasons, not simply due to insufficient knowledge.
We identified conspiracy beliefs particularly relating to the public being deceived about safety and effectiveness of vaccines, with stronger endorsement of deception beliefs associated with higher likelihood of holding misbeliefs. A 2020 US-based study reported that respondents who doubted the safety of COVID-19 vaccines had greater likelihood of having conspiracy beliefs and lower vaccine knowledge [39]. Other studies have reported an association between conspiracy beliefs about COVID and propensity to reject information from expert authorities [41], and reduced likelihood of following public health guidance [42,43,44], which can have negative consequences such as vaccine refusal.
Higher trust in scientific institutions and confidence in the government were associated with decreased susceptibility to misbeliefs about COVID-19 vaccines. Trust and confidence in medical and scientific experts, and government, have also been shown to be strong predictors of vaccine acceptance across countries and compliance with public health advice [45,46,47]. Trust in government and institutions was low in this study compared to our previous sample [15], which has been demonstrated in other studies more broadly in younger age samples, particularly in the context of confidence in political institutions (e.g., [48]).
Trust has been central in the context of COVID-19 and COVID-19 vaccines, as there has been, at times, significant uncertainty and incomplete evidence and information given the rapid speed with which the pandemic has progressed, and vaccine development response. In the first six months of vaccine rollout, Australia experienced delays in accessing vaccines produced overseas and uncoordinated delivery, which contributed to missed vaccine targets [49]. There was also vaccine hesitancy due to safety concerns over the AstraZeneca vaccine, which drew significant negative publicity; despite the low risk, people developed preferences for specific vaccines that were difficult to shift [50]. Our participants had more confidence in the safety and effectiveness of other vaccines such as travel and childhood vaccines than COVID-19 vaccines, indicating that there may be factors unique to COVID vaccines that determine people’s acceptance and reactions.
Confidence and trust in vaccine benefits and safety may be enhanced by fostering trust more broadly in the healthcare system, science, and government [51]. It is important to understand the role of trust in relation to vaccine misperceptions so that communications and interventions can be specifically designed and implemented in a way that will reach and appeal to groups that are distrustful of authorities developing, dispensing, and communicating about COVID vaccines. Interventions to strengthen public trust in science, increase information literacy, and combat misinformation are as important as ever.

7. Limitations

Population attitudes, motivations, and beliefs change rapidly and will likely have fluctuated throughout the various stages of the pandemic in response to public health campaigns and advice, vaccine availability and access, and community risk. It is important to regularly track factors that are associated with vaccination willingness to continue to inform communications and to ensure that public health campaigns are responsive to on-the-ground drivers of vaccine uptake.
This study was conducted in a high-income country at a single point in time, in the context of rapidly changing pandemic risk, information, and vaccination availability and rollout. The survey was distributed pre-vaccine rollout for this age group, so responses may have changed over time. Trust, for example, may have shifted as more information about vaccine safety and efficacy became available. Our measure of vaccine knowledge was validated but not specific to COVID-19 vaccines; this may be explored in future studies. Our measure of COVID-19 vaccine misbeliefs was developed specifically for the purposes of this study; one item was omitted from regression analyses (COVID vaccinated people can affect non-vaccinated people’s health) as we noted that the item could reasonably be interpreted as true (i.e., people vaccinated against COVID-19 can protect the health of others in the community).

8. Conclusions

Misbeliefs about COVID-19 vaccines are common in young adults in Australia, with two-thirds of participants harbouring at least one misbelief. Identifying misperceptions and gaps in knowledge about vaccines, such as those found in this study, provides valuable information for public health agencies to better address in vaccine communication initiatives. Additionally, understanding individual factors associated with vaccine misperceptions, including lower health literacy, less knowledge about vaccines, and lower confidence in government and science, can inform targeted public health messaging and the planning of communication strategies for future vaccine rollouts.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph19116883/s1, Table S1. Themes identified in free text responses to question “In the last week have you come across any content discouraging people from vaccinating”? (n = 963); Table S2: Sources identified in free text responses to question “In the last week have you come across any content discouraging people from vaccinating”? (n = 963); Table S3: Correlation matrix for study variables.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.P., T.C., K.J.M. and G.M.-K.; Methodology, all authors; Formal Analysis, G.M.-K., E.C., M.T. and B.C.; Investigation, all authors; Data Curation, G.M.-K., M.T. and B.C.; Writing—Original Draft Preparation, K.P., T.C., G.M.-K., R.H.D. and B.N.; Writing—Review and Editing, all authors; Funding Acquisition, K.P., T.C., K.J.M., G.M.-K., R.H.D., C.B., E.C., H.S. and M.S.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Australian Skeptics Inc. and the Sydney Institute for Infectious Diseases. The study funders had no role in writing of the manuscript or the decision to submit it for publication.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee of The University of Sydney (2020/212).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data are contained within the article and supplementary material. Any additional data are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

We thank our study funders for their support and all study participants.

Conflicts of Interest

H.S. reports funding from Sanofi Pasteur and Seqirus and honoraria from Sanofi Pasteur and Smartling. MSS reports funding to their organisation from NSW Health. G.M.-K. is a volunteer committee member of the Australian Skeptics Society. KM. receives funding from the National Health and Medical Research Council. All other authors declare no competing interest.

References

  1. Glassman, A.; Kenny, C.; Yang, G. COVID-19 Vaccine Development and Rollout in Historical Perspective; Center for Global Development: Washington, DC, USA, 2022.
  2. Dodd, R.H.; Cvejic, E.; Bonner, C.; Pickles, K.; McCaffery, K.J.; Ayre, J.; Batcup, C.; Copp, T.; Cornell, S.; Dakin, T. Willingness to Vaccinate against COVID-19 in Australia. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2020, 21, 318–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Dodd, R.H.; Pickles, K.; Nickel, B.; Cvejic, E.; Ayre, J.; Batcup, C.; Bonner, C.; Copp, T.; Cornell, S.; Dakin, T. Concerns and motivations about COVID-19 vaccination. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2021, 21, 161–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Dodd, R.H.; Pickles, K.; Cvejic, E.; Cornell, S.; Isautier, J.M.; Copp, T.; Nickel, B.; Bonner, C.; Batcup, C.; Muscat, D.M. Perceived public health threat a key factor for willingness to get the COVID-19 vaccine in Australia. Vaccine 2021, 40, 2484–2490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Tram, K.H.; Saeed, S.; Bradley, C.; Fox, B.; Eshun-Wilson, I.; Mody, A.; Geng, E. Deliberation, Dissent, and Distrust: Understanding Distinct Drivers of Coronavirus Disease 2019 Vaccine Hesitancy in the United States. Clin. Infect. Dis. 2021, 74, 1429–1441. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Rhodes, A.; Hoq, M.; Measey, M.-A.; Danchin, M. Intention to vaccinate against COVID-19 in Australia. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2021, 21, e110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Lin, C.; Tu, P.; Beitsch, L.M. Confidence and receptivity for COVID-19 vaccines: A rapid systematic review. Vaccines 2020, 9, 16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Yamey, G.; Garcia, P.; Hassan, F.; Mao, W.; McDade, K.K.; Pai, M.; Saha, S.; Schellekens, P.; Taylor, A.; Udayakumar, K. It is not too late to achieve global COVID-19 vaccine equity. Bmj 2022, 376, e070650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Pickles, K.; Copp, T.; Dodd, R.H.; Cvejic, E.; Seale, H.; Steffens, M.S.; Meyerowitz-Katz, G.; Bonner, C.; McCaffery, K. COVID-19 vaccine intentions in Australia. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2021, 21, 1627–1628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Larson, H.J. The biggest pandemic risk? Viral misinformation. Nature 2018, 562, 309–310. [Google Scholar]
  11. Loomba, S.; de Figueiredo, A.; Piatek, S.J.; de Graaf, K.; Larson, H.J. Measuring the impact of COVID-19 vaccine misinformation on vaccination intent in the UK and USA. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2021, 5, 337–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Kricorian, K.; Civen, R.; Equils, O. COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy: Misinformation and perceptions of vaccine safety. Hum. Vaccines Immunother. 2021, 18, 1950504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Abbasi, J. Widespread misinformation about infertility continues to create COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. JAMA 2022, 327, 1013–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Roozenbeek, J.; Schneider, C.R.; Dryhurst, S.; Kerr, J.; Freeman, A.L.; Recchia, G.; Van Der Bles, A.M.; Van Der Linden, S. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2020, 7, 201199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Pickles, K.; Cvejic, E.; Nickel, B.; Copp, T.; Bonner, C.; Leask, J.; Ayre, J.; Batcup, C.; Cornell, S.; Dakin, T. COVID-19 misinformation trends in Australia: Prospective longitudinal national survey. J. Med. Internet Res. 2021, 23, e23805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. WHO. World Health Organisation Coronavirus (COVID-19) Dashboard. 2022. Available online: https://covid19.who.int/region/wpro/country/au (accessed on 2 February 2022).
  17. Dyer, O. COVID-19: Australian outbreak surges as New Zealand sees first domestic cases in six months. BMJ Br. Med. J. 2021, 374, n2050. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Australian Government Department of Health. COVID-19 Vaccination Daily Rollout Update. Available online: https://www.health.gov.au/resources/collections/covid-19-vaccination-daily-rollout-update (accessed on 2 February 2022).
  19. Chew, L.D.; Griffin, J.M.; Partin, M.R.; Noorbaloochi, S.; Grill, J.P.; Snyder, A.; Bradley, K.A.; Nugent, S.M.; Baines, A.D.; Vanryn, M. Validation of screening questions for limited health literacy in a large VA outpatient population. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2008, 23, 561–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Norman, C.D.; Skinner, H.A. eHEALS: The eHealth literacy scale. J. Med. Internet Res. 2006, 8, e27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. Australian Government Department of Health. Is It True? Get the Facts on COVID-19 Vaccines. Available online: https://www.health.gov.au/initiatives-and-programs/covid-19-vaccines/is-it-true (accessed on 2 February 2022).
  22. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Myths and Facts about COVID-19 Vaccines; Centers for Disease Control and Prevention: Atlanta, GA, USA, 2021.
  23. Johns Hopkins Medicine. COVID-19 Vaccines: Myth Versus Fact. 2021. Available online: https://www.hopkinsmedicine.org/health/conditions-and-diseases/coronavirus/covid-19-vaccines-myth-versus-fact (accessed on 23 January 2022).
  24. Zingg, A.; Siegrist, M. Measuring people’s knowledge about vaccination: Developing a one-dimensional scale. Vaccine 2012, 30, 3771–3777. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Shapiro, G.K.; Holding, A.; Perez, S.; Amsel, R.; Rosberger, Z. Validation of the vaccine conspiracy beliefs scale. Papillomavirus Res. 2016, 2, 167–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Wolf, M.S.; Serper, M.; Opsasnick, L.; O’Conor, R.M.; Curtis, L.; Benavente, J.Y.; Wismer, G.; Batio, S.; Eifler, M.; Zheng, P. Awareness, attitudes, and actions related to COVID-19 among adults with chronic conditions at the onset of the US outbreak: A cross-sectional survey. Ann. Intern. Med. 2020, 173, 100–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Chow, M.Y.K.; Danchin, M.; Willaby, H.W.; Pemberton, S.; Leask, J. Parental attitudes, beliefs, behaviours and concerns towards childhood vaccinations in Australia: A national online survey. Aust. Fam. Physician 2017, 46, 145. [Google Scholar]
  28. Frew, P.M.; Murden, R.; Mehta, C.C.; Chamberlain, A.T.; Hinman, A.R.; Nowak, G.; Mendel, J.; Aikin, A.; Randall, L.A.; Hargreaves, A.L. Development of a US trust measure to assess and monitor parental confidence in the vaccine system. Vaccine 2019, 37, 325–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. WHO. Data for Action: Achieving High Uptake of COVID-19 Vaccines; WHO: Geneva, Switzerland, 2021.
  30. Allington, D.; Duffy, B.; Wessely, S.; Dhavan, N.; Rubin, J. Health-protective behaviour, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency. Psychol. Med. 2020, 51, 1763–1769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Weber, R.P. Basic Content Analysis; Sage: Newcastle upon Tyne, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  32. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Education and Work, Australia. May 2021. Available online: https://www.abs.gov.au/statistics/people/education/education-and-work-australia/latest-release (accessed on 4 March 2022).
  33. Melbourne Institute: Applied Economic & Social Research. Vaccine Hesitancy Tracker. Available online: https://melbourneinstitute.unimelb.edu.au/publications/research-insights/ttpn/vaccination-report (accessed on 4 March 2022).
  34. McCaffery, K.; Dodd, R.H.; Cvejic, E.; Ayre, J.; Batcup, C.; Isautier, J.M.; Copp, T.; Bonner, C.; Pickles, K.; Nickel, B. Disparities in COVID-19 related knowledge, attitudes, beliefs and behaviours by health literacy. Public Health Research & Practice 2020, 9, 30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Paasche-Orlow, M.K.; Parker, R.M.; Gazmararian, J.A.; Nielsen-Bohlman, L.T.; Rudd, R.R. The prevalence of limited health literacy. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 2005, 20, 175–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Sørensen, K.; Pelikan, J.M.; Röthlin, F.; Ganahl, K.; Slonska, Z.; Doyle, G.; Fullam, J.; Kondilis, B.; Agrafiotis, D.; Uiters, E. Health literacy in Europe: Comparative results of the European health literacy survey (HLS-EU). Eur. J. Public Health 2015, 25, 1053–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Larson, H.J. A call to arms: Helping family, friends and communities navigate the COVID-19 infodemic. Nat. Rev. Immunol. 2020, 20, 449–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. The Lancet Infectious Diseases. The COVID-19 infodemic. Lancet Infect. Dis. 2020, 20, 875. [CrossRef]
  39. Ruiz, J.B.; Bell, R.A. Predictors of intention to vaccinate against COVID-19: Results of a nationwide survey. Vaccine 2021, 39, 1080–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ratzan, S.C.; Parker, R.M. Vaccine Literacy—Helping Everyone Decide to Accept Vaccination. J. Health Commun. 2020, 25, 750–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Uscinski, J.E.; Enders, A.M.; Klofstad, C.; Seelig, M.; Funchion, J.; Everett, C.; Wuchty, S.; Premaratne, K.; Murthi, M. Why do people believe COVID-19 conspiracy theories? Harv. Kennedy Sch. Misinformation Rev. 2020, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Duffy, B.; Allington, D. COVID Conspiracies and Confusions: The Impact on Compliance with the UK’s Lockdown Rules and the Link with Social Media Use; The Policy Institute, King’s College: London, UK, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  43. Plohl, N.; Musil, B. Modeling compliance with COVID-19 prevention guidelines: The critical role of trust in science. Psychol. Health Med. 2021, 26, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Freeman, D.; Waite, F.; Rosebrock, L.; Petit, A.; Causier, C.; East, A.; Jenner, L.; Teale, A.-L.; Carr, L.; Mulhall, S. Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs, mistrust, and compliance with government guidelines in England. Psychol. Med. 2022, 52, 251–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Thaker, J. The persistence of vaccine hesitancy: COVID-19 vaccination intention in New Zealand. J. Health Commun. 2021, 26, 104–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Jennings, W.; Stoker, G.; Willis, H.; Valgardsson, V.; Gaskell, J.; Devine, D.; McKay, L.; Mills, M.C. Lack of trust and social media echo chambers predict COVID-19 vaccine hesitancy. MedRxiv 2021. [Google Scholar]
  47. Kerr, J.R.; Schneider, C.R.; Recchia, G.; Dryhurst, S.; Sahlin, U.; Dufouil, C.; Arwidson, P.; Freeman, A.L.; Van Der Linden, S. Correlates of intended COVID-19 vaccine acceptance across time and countries: Results from a series of cross-sectional surveys. BMJ Open 2021, 11, e048025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Fu, J.; Wyn, J.; Churchill, B. Young Australians’ Confidence in Political Institutions and Their Civic Engagement; The University of Melbourne: Melbourne, Australia, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  49. Zhou, N. Missed It by That Much: Australia Falls 3.4 m Doses Short of 4m Vaccination Target by End of March. The Guardian, 31 March 2021; p. 1. [Google Scholar]
  50. Biddle, N.; Edwards, B.; Gray, M.; Sollis, K. Vaccine Willingness and Concerns in Australia: August 2020 to April 2021; Centre for Social Research and Methods, Australian National University: Canberra, Australia, 2021.
  51. Larson, H.J.; Clarke, R.M.; Jarrett, C.; Eckersberger, E.; Levine, Z.; Schulz, W.S.; Paterson, P. Measuring trust in vaccination: A systematic review. Hum. Vaccines Immunother. 2018, 14, 1599–1609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Measures evaluated in this study.
Table 1. Measures evaluated in this study.
ItemDescription and Reference (If Applicable)Item Scoring and Analysis
Main outcome
COVID-19 vaccine related misbeliefs [21,22,23]Mean value 10 items; scale: 1 = definitely false to 4 = definitely true; 5 = I don’t know enough to make a decision
(Responses recoded from 1 (definitely false) to 5 (definitely true); 3 (I don’t know)
Getting the COVID-19 vaccine gives you COVID-19
More people will die of a negative side effect to the COVID-19 vaccine than would actually die from the virus
The flu shot provides immunity to COVID-19
Supplements are more effective than COVID-19 vaccines
COVID vaccines cause immune damage
People who have had a COVID-19 vaccine shed the virus to others
COVID vaccinated people can affect non-vaccinated people’s health *
COVID-19 vaccines have been linked to infertility
COVID-19 vaccines contain tracking technology
COVID-19 vaccines alter your DNA
Explanatory variables
General vaccine knowledge [24]Mean value 8 items (0–1): scale: Agree, don’t agree, don’t know (Scored as % correct)
Vaccines are not needed because diseases can be treated (e.g., with antibiotics)
Without broadly applied vaccine programs, smallpox would still exist
The effectiveness of vaccines has been proven
People would be more resistant to diseases if they were not given so many vaccines
Conditions like autism, multiple sclerosis, and diabetes might be triggered through vaccinations
The immune system is overloaded if we are given too many vaccinations
The doses of chemicals used in vaccines are not dangerous for humans
Vaccinations increase the occurrence of allergies
Acceptance of vaccine conspiracies [25]Mean value 7 items: scale 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree
Data about vaccine safety is often fabricated (made up)
People are deceived about the effectiveness of vaccines
Immunising is harmful, and this fact is covered up
Drug companies cover up the dangers of vaccines
Data about vaccine effectiveness is often fabricated (made up)
People are deceived about vaccine safety
The government is trying to cover up the link between vaccines and autism
Perceived risk of COVID-19Two individual items, adapted from [26]
Perceived public threat of COVID-19 (scale: 1 = no threat at all to 10 = very serious public health threat)
Concern about getting COVID-19 (scale: 1 = not at all concerned
to 4 = very concerned)
Confidence in GovernmentMean of 4 items, adapted from [27]; scale: 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree
I am confident in information about COVID-19 vaccines provided by the government
I am satisfied with the amount of information about COVID-19 vaccines provided by the government
I will follow government advice on COVID-19 vaccination to
help protect the wider community
I am concerned that government recommendations about
COVID-19 vaccines are not safe
Trust in InstitutionsMean of 3 items adapted from [28], scale: 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree
Scientists involved in developing and testing COVID-19 vaccines
Researchers involved in trialling COVID-19 vaccine safety and
efficacy
Medical institutions (GPs, hospitals) involved in distributing COVID-19 vaccines
Trust in GovernmentMean of 2 items adapted from [28], scale: 1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree
Federal government agencies responsible for managing the
control of COVID-19
State government agencies responsible for managing the
control of COVID-19
Confidence in COVID-19 vaccine benefits4 items, scale range 1–4, adapted from [29] (reported descriptively)
Perceived importance of COVID-19 vaccine for health
Perceived protection to self and community from getting
COVID-19 vaccine
Perceived safety of COVID-19 vaccine
Perceived concern about having a serious reaction to
COVID-19 vaccine
Frequency checking COVID-19 information
How often, if at all, do you check social media (such as Facebook or Twitter) for information or updates about COVID-19?1 item, scale: 1 = Once an hour or more to 5 = I don’t use social media, adapted from [30]
Exposure to negative information about COVID-19 vaccines
Apart from concerns about rare blood clots, have you seen or heard anything else bad about COVID-19 vaccines? —YesContent analysis, adapted from [29]
In the last week, have you come across any content discouraging people from vaccinating (i.e., on your social media, from friends, at the workplace?) —Yes
* This item was excluded from regression analyses, as we note that this item could reasonably be interpreted as true (i.e., people vaccinated against COVID-19 can protect the health of others in the community).
Table 2. Sample characteristics (N = 2050). Values are shown as n (%) unless otherwise indicated.
Table 2. Sample characteristics (N = 2050). Values are shown as n (%) unless otherwise indicated.
Characteristicn (%)
Age (years), mean (SD) 33.13 (7.9)
Highest level of educationHigh school or less460 (22.4%)
Certificate I-IV722 (35.2%)
University868 (42.3%)
GenderWoman1028 (50.1%)
Man1009 (49.2%)
Non-binary, transgender,
or prefer not to say #
11 (0.5%)
Residential state or territoryAustralian Capital Territory30 (1.5%)
Northern Territory17 (0.8%)
New South Wales619 (30.2%)
Victoria513 (25.0%)
Queensland439 (21.4%)
Western Australia214 (10.4%)
South Australia159 (7.8%)
Tasmania59 (2.9%)
Adequate health literacy 1707 (83.3%)
Digital health literacy (1–5), mean (SD) 3.8 (0.6)
Difficulty finding/understanding information online (1–10), mean (SD) 4.9 (1.9)
How serious of a public health threat do you think COVID-19 is in Australia, (1–10), mean (SD) 7.06 (2.52)
How concerned are you about getting COVID-19?Not at all concerned290 (14.1%)
A little concerned784 (38.2%)
Moderately concerned609 (29.7%)
Very concerned367 (17.9%)
Confidence in government (1–7), mean (SD) * 4.52 (1.4)
Trust in institutions (1–7), mean (SD) * 5.06 (1.5)
Trust in government (1–7), mean (SD) * 4.36 (1.7)
Information source about COVID-19 vaccines, %Official Government source53%
Mainstream media46%
Social media45%
* Higher scores indicate greater confidence and trust; # “non-binary” and “not given” had too few observations and were suppressed.
Table 3. Trust in vaccine effectiveness and safety.
Table 3. Trust in vaccine effectiveness and safety.
How Much Do You Trust *Effectiveness (n, %)Safety (n, %)
Childhood vaccines1760 (85.8)1801 (87.9)
Flu vaccine1537 (75.0)1624 (79.2)
Travel vaccines1506 (73.5)1510 (73.7)
Pfizer1363 (66.5)1329 (64.9)
AstraZeneca867 (42.3)792 (38.6)
* Combined “very much” and “moderately” versus all other responses (“a little” and “not at all”).
Table 4. Mean scores (SD) for misperceptions, knowledge, conspiracy.
Table 4. Mean scores (SD) for misperceptions, knowledge, conspiracy.
Variablen (%)
Vaccine misperceptions (1–5) a2.2 (0.9)
Vaccine-related conspiracy beliefs (1–5) b2.6 (0.9)
Vaccine knowledge (% correct) c49% (26%)
a Responses recoded from 1 (definitely false) to 5 (definitely true); 3 (uncertainty response). b Higher score indicating more agreement with conspiratorial beliefs. c Computed based on binary correct/incorrect responses.
Table 5. Agreement with COVID-19 vaccine misbeliefs.
Table 5. Agreement with COVID-19 vaccine misbeliefs.
TrueFalseDon’t Know Enough to Make a Decision
COVID vaccinated people can affect non-vaccinated people’s health *602 (29.4%)1081 (52.7%)367 (17.9%)
People who have had a COVID-19 vaccine shed the virus to others459 (22.4%)1185 (57.8%)406 (19.8%)
COVID-19 vaccines cause immune damage386 (18.8%)1172 (57.2%)492 (24.0%)
COVID-19 vaccines have been linked to infertility366 (17.9%)963 (47.0%)721 (35.2%)
More people will die of a negative side effect to the COVID-19 vaccine than would actually die from the virus354 (17.3%)1431 (69.8%)265 (12.9%)
COVID-19 vaccines alter your DNA324 (15.8%)1283 (62.6%)443 (21.6%)
Getting the COVID-19 vaccine gives you COVID-19308 (15.0%)1477 (72.1%)265 (12.9%)
Supplements are more effective than COVID-19 vaccines285 (13.9%)1472 (71.8%)293 (14.3%)
The flu shot provides immunity to COVID-19268 (13.1%)1561 (76.1%)221 (10.8%)
COVID-19 vaccines contain tracking technology253 (12.3%)1488 (72.6%)309 (15.1%)
* This item was excluded from regression analyses as we note that this item could reasonably be interpreted as true (i.e., people vaccinated against COVID-19 can protect the health of others in the community). We found roughly an equivalent % endorsement between the first and second most common misbeliefs (29% vs. 22% agreement)
Table 6. Vaccine-related conspiracy beliefs.
Table 6. Vaccine-related conspiracy beliefs.
AgreeDisagreeUnsure
People are deceived about the effectiveness of vaccines651 (31.8%)859 (41.9%)540 (26.3%)
People are deceived about vaccine safety607 (29.6%)877 (42.8%)566 (27.6%)
Drug companies cover up the dangers of vaccines571 (27.9%)848 (41.4%)631 (30.8%)
Data about vaccine safety is often fabricated (made up)445 (21.7%)898 (43.8%)707 (34.5%)
Data about vaccine effectiveness is often fabricated (made up)434 (21.2%)994 (48.5%)622 (30.3%)
The government is trying to cover up the link between vaccines and autism308 (15.0%)1105 (53.9%)637 (31.1%)
Immunising is harmful, and this fact is covered up280 (13.7%)1284 (62.6%)486 (23.7%)
Table 7. General vaccine knowledge—Shading indicates correct response.
Table 7. General vaccine knowledge—Shading indicates correct response.
AgreeDisagreeDon’t Know
The effectiveness of vaccines has been proven1629 (79.5%)212 (10.3%)209 (10.2%)
Vaccines are not needed because diseases can be treated (e.g., with antibiotics)235 (11.5%)1619 (79.0%)196 (9.6%)
Without broadly applied vaccine programs, smallpox would still exist1507 (73.5%)200 (9.8%)343 (16.7%)
People would be more resistant to diseases if they were not given so many vaccines384 (18.7%)1113 (54.3%)553 (27.0%)
Conditions like autism, multiple sclerosis, and diabetes might be triggered through vaccinations347 (16.9%)1015 (49.5%)688 (33.6%)
The immune system is overloaded if we are given too many vaccinations473 (23.1%)904 (44.1%)673 (32.8%)
The doses of chemicals used in vaccines are not dangerous for humans829 (40.4%)504 (24.6%)717 (35.0%)
Vaccinations increase the occurrence of allergies351 (17.1%)753 (36.7%)946 (46.1%)
Table 8. Multivariable truncated linear regression of the misbeliefs score. Higher values of the outcome indicate greater support for misbeliefs. Data are presented as estimated marginal mean differences (95% CIs). Statistical significance is indicated by * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001. N = 2050 complete cases.
Table 8. Multivariable truncated linear regression of the misbeliefs score. Higher values of the outcome indicate greater support for misbeliefs. Data are presented as estimated marginal mean differences (95% CIs). Statistical significance is indicated by * p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01; *** p < 0.001. N = 2050 complete cases.
Main Effect p-ValueBase Model (Control Variables Only)Main Effect p-ValueEstimated Marginal Mean Differences (Full Multivariable Model)
Control Variables
Age (/year) 0.008 **
[0.003,0.013]
0.001
[−0.002,0.004]
Male gender (vs. female) a 0.106 **
[0.029,0.183]
−0.013
[−0.066,0.041]
Non-binary, transgender −0.276
[−0.628,0.076]
Prefer not to say 0.812
[−0.344,1.968]
Education (vs. high school or less)<0.0001 0.01
Cert I-IV 0.018
[−0.087,0.123]
−0.018
[−0.091,0.053]
University education −0.315 ***
[−0.416,−0.214]
−0.108 **
[−0.178,−0.038]
State (vs. NSW)0.087−0.132
[−0.452,0.188]
0.094
ACT −0.120
[−0.444,0.204]
0.063
[−0.157,0.282]
NT −0.124
[−0.556,0.309]
0.026
[−0.268,0.320]
VIC 0.043
[−0.061,0.146]
0.012
[−0.059,0.083]
QLD −0.063
[−0.045,0.171]
0.063
[−0.012,0.138]
WA −0.099
[−0.237,0.037]
−0.049
[−0.145,0.046]
SA −0.137
[−0.290,0.016]
−0.098
[−0.204,0.008]
TAS 0.0759
[−0.159,0.311]
−0.039
[−0.200,0.120]
Vaccine knowledge −0.779 ***
[−0.912,−0.646]
Health literacy −0.128 ***
[−0.200,−0.056]
Confidence in government −0.181 ***
[−0.241,−0.122]
Conspiratorial beliefs 0.301 ***
[0.263,0.340]
Trust in institutions −0.147 ***
[−0.176,−0.117]
Trust in governments 0.016
[−0.007,0.038]
Perceived personal risk of COVID-19 0.033 *
[0.0004,0.065]
Frequency checking social media for COVID-19 information or updates −0.027 *
[−0.049,−0.005]
a Marginal mean differences are not reported for gender reported as “not specified” or “other” due to small sample size, but these data were included in the regression model.
Table 9. Content discouraging COVID-19 vaccination that participants had witnessed, with frequency of the themes and example quotes.
Table 9. Content discouraging COVID-19 vaccination that participants had witnessed, with frequency of the themes and example quotes.
Themen (%)Example Free Text Response
Negative vaccine side effects and vaccine risk384 (39.9)“I heard that getting vaccinated can cause rare blood clots and is dangerous so I choose not to, I don’t want to risk my life”
Conspiracy theories133 (13.8)“Tin foil hat people saying the vaccine is designed to make you more likely to catch a virus which will be released in 2025 as the world governments want to cull the population by 6.5 billion people”
Antivax or hesitance towards vaccine115 (11.9)“Many people within my community are strongly against the vaccine for many well informed and educated reasons”
Concerns about lack of vaccine testing and contents75 (7.8)“The immunization has been rushed way to quickly without adequate testing before it was released to the public”
Distrust in government64 (6.6)“Gov’t not reporting the cases of adverse effect and death due to COVID-19 vaccines”
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Pickles, K.; Copp, T.; Meyerowitz-Katz, G.; Dodd, R.H.; Bonner, C.; Nickel, B.; Steffens, M.S.; Seale, H.; Cvejic, E.; Taba, M.; et al. COVID-19 Vaccine Misperceptions in a Community Sample of Adults Aged 18–49 Years in Australia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 6883. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116883

AMA Style

Pickles K, Copp T, Meyerowitz-Katz G, Dodd RH, Bonner C, Nickel B, Steffens MS, Seale H, Cvejic E, Taba M, et al. COVID-19 Vaccine Misperceptions in a Community Sample of Adults Aged 18–49 Years in Australia. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022; 19(11):6883. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116883

Chicago/Turabian Style

Pickles, Kristen, Tessa Copp, Gideon Meyerowitz-Katz, Rachael H. Dodd, Carissa Bonner, Brooke Nickel, Maryke S. Steffens, Holly Seale, Erin Cvejic, Melody Taba, and et al. 2022. "COVID-19 Vaccine Misperceptions in a Community Sample of Adults Aged 18–49 Years in Australia" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 19, no. 11: 6883. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph19116883

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop