Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 13 January 2023
Sec. Psychopathology

A longitudinal approach to understanding boredom during pandemics: The predictive roles of trauma and emotion dysregulation

  • Boredom Lab, Department of Psychology, York University, Toronto, ON, Canada

Research during the COVID-19 pandemic and prior outbreaks suggest that boredom is linked to poor compliance with critical lifesaving social distancing and quarantine guidelines, as well as to numerous mental health difficulties. As such, continued understanding on what contributes to boredom is imperative. Extending beyond the roles of constraint, monotony, and trait dispositions (e.g., individual differences in boredom propensity), and informed by prior theories on the emotional contributors of boredom, the current longitudinal study examined the predictive role of “pandemic trauma” on people’s boredom, with a focus on how emotion dysregulation mediates this relationship. Community participants (N = 345) completed questionnaires three times across an average of 3 1/2 weeks, rating their pandemic trauma, emotion dysregulation, and boredom over the past week each time. Pandemic trauma was assessed with items querying exposure to coronavirus, as well as the financial, resource-related, and interpersonal pandemic stressors that participants experienced. Emotion dysregulation was assessed with the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale. Boredom was assessed with the short-form Multidimensional State Boredom Scale. The results of a theory-informed mediation model showed that participants’ pandemic trauma at Time 1 positively and modestly predicted their boredom at Time 3 and that this relationship was partially and moderately mediated by participants’ lack of emotional clarity and difficulties with engaging in goal-directed behaviors at Time 2. When people experience pandemic-related trauma, they subsequently struggle to understand their feelings and engage in goal-oriented actions, and, in turn, feel more bored. Theoretical and clinical implications as related to the emotional underpinnings of boredom are discussed.

1. Introduction

The coronavirus (COVID-19) outbreak is a global pandemic that began in March 2020. Research suggests that traumatic stress symptoms (e.g., intrusive re-experiencing, heightened arousal) have been particularly prevalent during the pandemic (Cooke et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2021). Notably, one study by Bridgland et al. (2021) found that participants across the United States, Canada, Australia, New Zealand, and United Kingdom who had come into contact with the coronavirus, lost work/income, had trouble buying supplies, experienced lockdown directives that limited their contact with others, or had experienced changes related to their children/dependents endorsed higher PTSD symptoms than those who did not have these experiences, which suggests that a pandemic can be understood as a traumatic stressor. The COVID-19 pandemic may be the deadliest viral outbreak in more than a century, but emerging research suggests a high and increasingly probability of observing pandemics similar to COVID-19 in the future (e.g., Marani et al., 2021). As such, it is critical to continue to better understand the effects of pandemics, and associated health measures, on people’s emotional wellbeing. The current study examined boredom, the aversive feeling of wanting, but being unable, to engage in satisfying activity (Fahlman et al., 2013), as an index of emotional wellbeing. Previous studies investigating boredom during pandemics have mainly focused on the external constraints and monotony imposed by the pandemic, as well as individual differences in trait dispositions toward boredom, as explanations for this aversive experience. The current study meaningfully extended this prior work, using a longitudinal design to examine the predictive roles of trauma and emotion dysregulation on boredom during the COVID-19 pandemic. Below, we review prior literature on the experience of boredom during pandemics, and then examine the theoretical and empirical literature on the relations of trauma, emotion dysregulation, and boredom.

1.1. The experience of boredom during pandemics

Boredom is often triggered by external factors, such as lack of choice, monotony, inappropriate levels of challenge, and devalued activities (Elpidorou, 2018). The feeling can be such a distressing state that people engage in unhealthy behaviors in order to alleviate it (e.g., self-harm, unhealthy eating; Havermans et al., 2015) and seek experiences that elicit a different emotional state—even if that state is negative (Bench and Lench, 2019). Boredom proneness (BP), characterized as the tendency to feel bored more frequently and intensely (Farmer and Sundberg, 1986; Tam et al., 2021), is also related to a host of psychosocial and mental health problems, such as depression, anxiety, substance abuse, addictive behaviors, and risk-taking (see Danckert et al., 2018 for a review).

In the context of a pandemic, people are asked to accept constraints and restrictions on normal behaviors, leaving them with a limited set of available activities that, for many, are monotonous, uninteresting, and void of meaning (Maison et al., 2021). Unsurprisingly, research conducted during the COVID-19 outbreak and the 2003 SARS outbreak suggests that boredom is one of the most commonly experienced feelings (e.g., Reynolds et al., 2008; Barari et al., 2020; Droit-Volet et al., 2020; Li et al., 2021; Martarelli et al., 2021; Martinelli et al., 2021; Wessels et al., 2022), as well as that boredom is a key emotional disincentive to complying with social distancing/quarantine guidelines (e.g., DiGiovanni et al., 2004; Wolff et al., 2020; Bieleke et al., 2021; Boylan et al., 2021; Brosowsky et al., 2021; Drody et al., 2022; but see Westgate et al., 2022 for their emerging findings related to pandemic boredom and risky public health behaviors). Equally important, the feeling of boredom and BP have been linked to various psychological problems among adolescents and adults during the pandemic, such as increased alcohol and substance use (e.g., vaping and smoking), problematic social media and Internet use (i.e., Internet addiction), perceived stress, and psychological distress, including symptoms of depression, anxiety, and insomnia (see Bambrah et al., 2022 for a review). Given the importance of social distancing/quarantine policies in curbing the spread of infectious diseases (e.g., Glogowsky et al., 2021) and the troubling relations between boredom and mental health during COVID-19, it is critical to understand the feeling of boredom as an outcome in its own right.

To date, research (Reynolds et al., 2008; Barari et al., 2020; Droit-Volet et al., 2020; Martarelli et al., 2021; Martinelli et al., 2021; Wessels et al., 2022) has largely underscored constraint, monotony, and lack of challenge imposed by the pandemic, as well as individual differences in BP, as contributors to the feeling of boredom during the COVID-19 and SARS outbreaks. Understandably, the implications of this work highlight integrating adventure, variety, challenge, and excitement in one’s day-to-day life so as to mitigate boredom. For example, colloquial discussion among media outlets suggests various things that people can do and alter within their external environments to “combat” the constraint and monotony associated with boredom (e.g., Ehl, 2021). The current longitudinal study extended this past work by examining two relevant, yet under-studied, predictors of boredom during the COVID-19 outbreak: trauma and emotion dysregulation.

1.2. Trauma and boredom

A small body of research has found a positive relationship between trauma and boredom. However, this literature as a whole can be characterized disparate and disconnected, as there is a lack of consistency across studies in how boredom is conceptualized and operationalized. In particular, some studies conceptualize boredom as a problematic, but transient, affective experience (i.e., Stretch et al., 1996a, 1996b; Silove et al., 1997; Pfefferbaum et al., 2007; Schweitzer et al., 2018), whereas other studies conceptualize boredom as a trait disposition (i.e., Chaney and Blalock, 2006; Volkert et al., 2013), but operationalize this with different measures (e.g., Boredom Proneness Scale by Farmer and Sundberg, 1986, vs. the Boredom Susceptibility Scale by Zuckerman, 1979) that are known to be associated with distinct constructs (Mercer-Lynn et al., 2013a, 2013b), thereby highlighting the discrepancy in how trait boredom is conceptualized. Additionally, the types of trauma examined across prior studies are variable, with studies examining boredom among refugee asylum-seekers (Silove et al., 1997; Schweitzer et al., 2018), Gulf War Veterans (Stretch et al., 1996a, 1996b), men with a history childhood abuse (Chaney and Blalock, 2006), adult victims of single or repeated interpersonal trauma (sexual and/or physical trauma; Volkert et al., 2013), and children after the 2001 World Trade Center attacks (Pfefferbaum et al., 2007). Most notably, a clear understanding on why there is a positive association between trauma and boredom is lacking from prior work, with some of the studies providing no explanation for the link (i.e., Stretch et al., 1996a, 1996b; Chaney and Blalock, 2006) and other studies providing distinct ideas for the link (e.g., social withdrawal, social exclusion, increased need for stimulation; Silove et al., 1997; Pfefferbaum et al., 2007; Volkert et al., 2013; Schweitzer et al., 2018). Accordingly, the current study not only sought to investigate the link between trauma and boredom as a transient state within the specific context of COVID-19, but also advance our understanding of this link by investigating the mediating role of emotion dysregulation. Our proposal that emotion dysregulation might mediate the relation between trauma and boredom is reinforced by the well-documented literature that suggests that trauma can lead to poor emotion regulation and the emerging understanding that poor emotion regulation might be an internal cause of boredom.

1.3. Trauma and emotion dysregulation

With respect to the link between trauma and emotion dysregulation, using Gratz and Roemer’s (2004) integrative conceptualization and corresponding measure, the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS), Ehring and Quack (2010) found that, among a large sample of trauma survivors, PTSD symptom severity was robustly associated with the lack of emotional awareness, lack of emotional clarity, difficulties with engaging in goal-directed behaviors, impulse control difficulties, nonacceptance of emotions, and the belief that there is little that can be done to regulate emotions effectively. Another more recent study (Mekawi et al., 2020) found that people with current PTSD reported more dysregulation across all six DERS subscales, relative to people without PTSD and with lifetime PTSD. This work suggests that people with trauma struggle to attend to, understand, control, and accept their negative emotions, as well as struggle to behave in a manner that is consistent with their goals and lack belief in their ability to effectively regulate their emotions. In other words, when people experience such wide-ranging emotion dysregulation, they often have a limited understanding of what they are feeling and fail to respond accordingly and appropriately.

1.4. Emotion dysregulation and boredom

A rationale for why poor emotion regulation might lead to subsequent boredom can be found in psychodynamic and existential theories of boredom. For example, psychodynamic theorists posit that boredom is a side-effect of defensive processes designed to keep verboten desires out of awareness. That is, attempts to keep specific desires out of awareness result in a more diffuse emotional numbing, lack of emotional clarity, and detachment, which, in turn, results in boredom because it is then difficult to determine what one would like to do. Furthermore, attempts to find satisfaction in the external world are never completely satisfying when one’s desires remain unconscious (Fenichel, 1953; Greenson, 1953; Wangh, 1975). Similarly, one strand of existential thought posits that boredom, and its associated lack of emotional clarity, serves as a defense against the overwhelming responsibility of having to continuously actualize oneself through engagement with the world (e.g., White, 1998; Bargdill, 2000). Thus, although different in detail, both psychodynamic and existential theories suggest boredom results when anxiety prevents people from bringing their predicament into focus and prevents people from ‘owning up to’ or ‘putting on the line’ what they desire and who they might become. Because of anxiety of the alternative, people settle for an emotionally numb and disengaged existence and ensuing boredom.

A few correlational studies lend empirical support to the notion that lack of emotional clarity and difficulties with emotion regulation might cause boredom, but, notably, these studies examine the link between dispositional indices of emotion dysregulation and boredom proneness (BP). For example, studies have found that people who possess a restricted capacity for labeling and describing their emotions are more prone to boredom (Seib and Vodanovich, 1998; Gana et al., 2000; Harris, 2000; von Gemmingen et al., 2003; Eastwood et al., 2007). Other work indicates that certain self-regulatory styles that diminish goal pursuit are associated with BP. For example, early work suggests that “state-orientation,” a change-preventing mode of self-regulation, is linked to boredom, specifically finding that BP is positively related to state-oriented “preoccupation” and “hesitation”—the former characterized by intrusive and persevering thoughts associated with unpleasant experiences and the latter characterized by an impaired initiation of an intended action (Blunt and Pychyl, 1998). Other studies have found that BP is positively correlated with the “Assessment” mode of self-regulation, which is characterized by a ruminative evaluative orientation (“do the right thing”) that hampers goal pursuit, and is negatively correlated with the “Locomotion” mode of self-regulation, which is characterized by an action-oriented mode (“just do it”) of goal pursuit (Struk et al., 2015; Mugon et al., 2018).

1.5. Current study

It is frequently reported that pandemics (e.g., the SARS outbreak, the COVID-19 pandemic) are associated with increased boredom. Yet, the majority of this empirical and colloquial discussion tends to be descriptive (e.g., documenting the occurrence and intensity of boredom) and prescriptive (e.g., offering ways to cope with boredom) in nature. Moreover, existing work primarily implicates the roles of pandemic-imposed constraint and monotony, and trait dispositions (i.e., BP) in people’s boredom. These prior studies set the stage for a timely and pragmatic research agenda that utilizes rigorous longitudinal methods and analysis to form a novel understanding of boredom by examining other possible contributors, namely, pandemic-related trauma mediated through emotion dysregulation. Driven by the above-reviewed theories of boredom, and by the empirical work on the emotional and self-regulatory correlates of boredom, the current study used a longitudinal design to explore the predictive role of trauma on people’s boredom during COVID-19, with a focus on how emotion dysregulation mediates this relationship. Drawing on the above-reviewed theories, we propose that people respond to pandemic trauma with regulatory strategies characterized by a disconnect from one’s emotional experiences, which, in turn, lead to increased feelings of boredom. Across three time points, we hypothesized that pandemic trauma (at Time 1) would predict greater emotion dysregulation (at Time 2), which, in turn, would predict greater boredom (at Time 3) among people in the general population. Beyond the COVID-19 pandemic, understanding the internal (i.e., unrelated to an impoverished external environment) and emotional contributors of boredom is a critical endeavor, as it may inform the development of interventions for people who chronically struggle with boredom and the wide-ranging mental health problems associated with it.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Participants and procedures

Ethics approval was obtained from our academic institution’s research ethics board. All participants were recruited from Qualtrics’ Online Panels, which provide community participants with opportunities to complete online studies. All participants provided informed consent before each data collection time point and Qualtrics compensated participants at each time point with a choice of different gift cards for various American and Canadian retailers. A full description of participant recruitment, including how we determined our sample size, participant attrition, and data exclusions, is available publicly1 and is reported elsewhere (Bambrah et al., 2022). Broadly we recruited participants who resided in the United States (U.S.) and Canada. Participants were contacted three times (via Qualtrics’ Online Panels) at the height of the COVID-19 pandemic, between 6 May 2020 and 18 June 2020. At each time point, participants completed a series of questionnaires about their experiences over the past week. At Time 1, participants completed additional measures about their background (e.g., demographic characteristics and pre-existing life stressors). Qualtrics’ Online Panels services mistakenly failed to invite all of the participants who had completed Time 1 to subsequently complete Time 2 questionnaires. Therefore, we recruited additional participants in a second wave to achieve an adequate sample size; collapsing across Wave 1 and Wave 2, the average attrition was 56% from Time 1 to Time 2 and 26% from Time 2 to Time 3. After removing participants who completed questionnaires in an unrealistically short or long time and/or participants who provided similar responses across positively and negatively worded items on questionnaires, the final sample at Time 3 across Waves 1 and 2 consisted of 345 participants (67.83% female), with a mean age of 49.26 years (SD = 16.68, range = 18–88). The majority of participants retained within the final sample resided in the U.S. (Wave 1 = 100.00%, Wave 2 = 99.68%).

See Table 1 for the demographic characteristics of the sample. The majority of participants reported living with someone else at the time of the study (N = 252, 73.04%); a romantic partner/spouse (61.90%) and children (42.46%) were the most frequently reported cohabitors. The majority of participants were not enrolled in school at the time of the study (N = 311, 90.14%). Of those participants who indicated current student enrollment (N = 34, 9.86%), more than half were completing courses online (52.94%). The highest level of education for the majority of participants was a high school diploma/GED (N = 156, 45.22%). Nearly two-thirds of participants were not currently employed at the time of the study (N = 219, 63.48%). Of those participants who indicated current employment (N = 126, 36.52%), the majority were working from home (42.06%). The majority of participants reported a yearly household income of $50,000 or less (N = 207, 60.00%).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample characteristics (N = 345).

Our sample included participants from 47 states in the U.S. The most highly represented states, specifically consisting of 20 or more participants, were Florida (9.01% of the U.S. sample), California (8.14%), New York (7.27%), and Texas (6.69%). Confirmed COVID-19 cases in the U.S. rose from 1.2 million on 6 May 2020 to 2.2 million on 18 June 2020, an increase of 1 million cases during this period of data collection (Johns Hopkins University Coronavirus Research Center, 2020). The total number of deaths related to COVID-19 rose from 76.9 thousand on 6 May 2020 to 121.1 thousand on 18 June 2020, an increase of 44.2 thousand deaths (Johns Hopkins University Coronavirus Research Center, 2020). In the month prior to the study (April 2020), the unemployment rate in the U.S. increased by 10.3% points to 14.7%—the highest rate and the largest over-the-month increase in the history of the data collected by the U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics (2020a). Unemployment rose sharply among all major worker groups. In May 2020, the unemployment rate declined slightly to 13.3%, reflecting a limited resumption of economic activity that was curtailed in March and April due to efforts to contain the pandemic (U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2020b).

2.2. Measures

Participants completed a large number of questionnaires of which a subset of responses are reported here. See Table 2 for the coefficient omega estimates and descriptive statistics (range, mean, standard deviation) of the measures that were administered at Times 1, 2, and 3 of the current study.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Coefficient omega estimates and descriptive statistics for continuous measured variables at Times 1, 2, and 3 (N = 345).

2.2.1. Time 1

2.2.1.1. Demographics

Participants reported their age, gender, country of residence (U.S. or Canada), living arrangement, educational background and enrollment status, employment status, and yearly household income (see Table 1 for more details).

2.2.1.2. Pre-existing stressors

Participants responded to one question that queried the number of burdensome life stressors—preceding the coronavirus pandemic—that they had experienced. Drawn from the Adjustment Disorder–New Module 20 Questionnaire (ADNM-20; Einsle et al., 2010), participants were provided a list of 16 stressful life events (e.g., assault) and they were asked to indicate which event(s), if any, happened to them during the past 2 years before the pandemic and remains a “very strong” burden to them/has burdened them in the past 6 months. Participants were also able to report stressful experiences that were not included on the list. A higher total score indicated a greater number of burdensome pre-existing (pre-pandemic) stressors. As shown in Table 1, the greatest proportion of participants (N = 115, 33.33%) reported at least one life stressor that preceded the COVID-19 pandemic and remains a burden for them. Financial problems (N = 124, 35.94%), death of a loved one (N = 84, 24.35%), and family conflicts (N = 83, 24.06%) were the three most frequently reported burdensome pre-existing stressors in the sample.

2.2.2. Times 1 to 3

2.2.2.1. Pandemic trauma

Drawing on the above-described findings from Bridgland et al. (2021), we assessed participants’ pandemic trauma with four indicator variables: (1) exposure to coronavirus (e.g., personal coronavirus diagnosis/symptoms and proximity to others with a coronavirus diagnosis/symptoms), as well as the (2) financial (e.g., losing job-related income), (3) resource-related (e.g., difficulty getting necessary supplies), and (4) interpersonal (e.g., difficulty socially connecting) stressors of the pandemic. The first three variables were measured using subscales from the Coronavirus Experiences Questionnaire and Coronavirus Impacts Questionnaire developed by Conway et al. (2020) and the fourth variable was measured using four items created by the authors of the current study. A total of 12 items were rated using a 7-point scale (1 = not true of me at all to 7 = very true of me). The exposure, financial, resource-related, and interpersonal subscales had acceptable-to-excellent internal reliability across all three time points (see Table 2 for the coefficient omega estimates of all measures administered between Times 1 and 3).

2.2.2.2. Emotion dysregulation

The 18-item DERS (DERS-18) is composed of the items that load most strongly onto their respective subscale from the original measure (i.e., all factor loadings > 0.75; Victor and Klonsky, 2016) and was used to assess participants’ emotion dysregulation over the past week: (1) clarity (i.e., not knowing the emotions that one is experiencing); (2) goals (i.e., difficulties concentrating and accomplishing goals/tasks when experiencing negative emotions); (3) impulse (i.e., difficulties remaining in control of one’s behavior when experiencing negative emotions); (4) nonacceptance (i.e., having negative secondary emotional responses to one’s negative emotions or nonaccepting reactions to one’s distress); and (5) strategies (i.e., believing that there is little that can be done to regulate one’s emotions effectively once upset). The awareness subscale was excluded from the current study, as emerging research shows that a revised five-factor model that excludes the subscale provides a better fit to the data and that the subscale possesses low associations with the other DERS subscales (e.g., Hallion et al., 2018 for a review). Participants rated each item using a 5-point scale [1 = almost never (0%–10%) to 5 = almost always (91%–100%)], with a higher subscale score indicating greater difficulties with that facet of emotion regulation over the past week. The subscales had good-to-excellent internal consistency across all three time points.

2.2.2.3. Boredom

The short-form Multidimensional State Boredom Scale (MSBS) assessed participants’ boredom over the past week. From the 28-item MSBS (Fahlman et al., 2013), participants rated six items using a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree to 7 = strongly agree), with a higher total score indicating greater boredom over the past week. A series of confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) conducted with undergraduate and community samples from other studies suggests that a one-factor structure of the six-item MSBS fits the data well and has good internal consistency: CFI range = 0.956 to 0.989, RMSEA range = 0.052 to 0.086, SRMR range = 0.025 to 0.045, and ω range = 0.79 to 0.85 (e.g., Eastwood and Bambrah, 2021). A CFA confirmed that a one-factor structure of the six-item MSBS had an acceptable-to-good fit with the data of the current study across all three time points: CFI range = 0.961 to 0.981, RMSEA range = 0.079 to 0.107, and SRMR range = 0.028 to 0.042. The six-item MSBS also had good internal reliability across all time points.

2.3. Data analysis

Data were analyzed using R (version 4.2.1; R Core Team, 2022), with the packages car (version 3.1–0; Fox and Weisberg, 2019), ggplot2 (version 3.3.6; Wickham, 2016), lavaan (version 0.6–12; Rosseel, 2012), boot (version 1.3–28; Canty and Ripley, 2021), and MBESS (version 4.9.1; Kelley, 2007).

2.3.1. Mediation analyses

To examine our hypotheses, we specified a structural equation model with one latent variable and seven observed variables using the lavaan package (Rosseel, 2012). The independent variable in the model was the latent variable of pandemic trauma at Time 1, as assessed by participants’ exposure to coronavirus, as well as the financial, resource-related, and interpersonal stressors of the pandemic. We conducted a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) to verify that a one-factor structure of the latent variable fit the data well; these results are presented in Figure 1. The mediator variables in the model were participants’ emotion dysregulation at Time 2, as measured by the five subscales of the DERS-18 (clarity, goals, impulse, nonacceptance, and strategies); these five subscales were set to covary in the model. The dependent variable in the model was boredom at Time 3, as measured by the total score of the short-form MSBS. Participants’ pre-existing stress was included as a covariate. As it is plausible that participants with more burdensome stressors prior to the pandemic would experience greater pandemic trauma, greater emotion dysregulation, and greater boredom, the pandemic trauma latent variable (at Time 1), the DERS-18 subscale scores (at Time 2), and the short-form MSBS total score (at Time 3) were regressed on to this covariate. These variables were also regressed on to age and gender, which were included as covariates. The hypothesized mediation model is depicted in Figure 2.2

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Model of pandemic trauma latent variable (at Time 1, N = 1,356). Unstandardized estimates are shown with standard errors in parentheses and with the 95% bias-corrected CI in brackets. The latent variable accounted for 27.90%, 38.10%, 51.40%, and 49.70% of the variable in the exposure to coronavirus, financial stress, resource-related stress, and interpersonal stress indicators, respectively. All p’s < 0.001.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Hypothesized mediation model predicting boredom (N = 344). Hypothesized mediation model (N = 344) of the pathways to boredom during the coronavirus pandemic. Unstandardized estimates with standard errors in parentheses are shown for all of the paths. Pre-pandemic stress, age, and gender were included as covariates, but are not depicted in the figure (see “Results” for findings related to these covariates). All five mediators at Time 2 were set to covary with each other, but this is not depicted in the model. ***p < 0.001; **p < 0.01; *p < 0.05.

The robust maximum likelihood estimator (MLR) was used to accommodate violations of the normality assumption. We used the following criteria as an indication of good model fit: comparative fit index (CFI) > 0.95; root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.06; and standardized root mean square residual (SRMR) < 0.08 (Hu and Bentler, 1999; as cited in Lai and Green, 2016); CFI values between 0.90 and 0.95 and RMSEA values between 0.05 and 0.10 suggest “acceptable” fit (Browne and Cudeck, 1992; MacCallum et al., 1996; McDonald and Ho, 2002; as cited in Lai and Green, 2016). If the model did not fit the data well, we planned to examine modification indices for potential sources of misfit and respecify the model. Given the post-hoc nature of this process, we planned to only respecify the model based on modification indices > 10 that made conceptual sense within our theoretical framework. Once the final mediation model was specified, bootstrapping (with 5,000 samples) was used to estimate the standard errors and 95% bias-corrected confidence intervals (CIs) for all unstandardized path coefficients (a, b, c’, and indirect) in the mediation model. Following recommendations by Steinberg and Thissen (2006) and Pek and Flora (2018) on the reporting of effect sizes in original psychological research, including structural equation models, we present and discuss the unstandardized parameter estimates within the mediation model.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive analyses

A series of Independent t-tests found no statistically significant differences between participants who were recruited in Wave 1 (n = 36) and participants who were recruited in Wave 2 (n = 309) on the four pandemic trauma indicator variables at Time 1; the five DERS-18 subscale scores at Time 2; the short-form MSBS total score across at Time 3; age; and the number of burdensome pre-existing (pre-pandemic) stressors reported (all p’s > 0.05). A Chi-square test also found no statistically significant difference between groups on gender (i.e., 1 = male, 2 = female; the one participant who did not report their gender was excluded from data analyses). Thus, all retained participants at Time 3 were analyzed together. Histograms and boxplots showed that the continuous variables in the hypothesized mediation model reasonably approximated normal distributions. Bivariate scatterplots showed linear relationships between all variables specified in the mediation model (i.e., the four indicators of pandemic trauma, the five facets of emotion dysregulation, boredom, and the covariates).

3.2. Mediation analyses

3.2.1. Pandemic trauma latent variable

Applying a CFA to scores for (a) exposure to coronavirus, (b) financial, (c) resource-related, and (d) interpersonal stressors of the pandemic, we created a latent variable of pandemic trauma, which served as the independent variable for the hypothesized mediation model. See Figure 1 for the unstandardized factor loadings and corresponding 95% bias-corrected CIs. As expected, a one-factor structure of these four indicators (among retained participants who completed Time 1, N = 1,356) fit the data well: χ2(2) = 9.929, p = 0.007, CFI = 0.994, RMSEA = 0.051, and SRMR = 0.016. The latent variable accounted for between 27.90 and 51.40% of the variance in the indicators and it accounted for significant variance in all of the indicators (all p’s < 0.001 for the factor loadings).

3.2.2. Hypothesized mediation model

Table 3 presents the zero-order correlations between all key measured variables in the mediation model. The hypothesized mediation model (N = 344; one participant who did not report their gender was excluded from this analysis) demonstrated an acceptable-to-good fit with the data, χ2 (30) = 97.728, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.966, RMSEA = 0.083, and SRMR = 0.043, and it was used as the final model as no modification indices met our criteria for consideration. The model accounted for 46.00% of the variance in boredom at Time 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Zero-order correlations between indicator, mediator, and outcome variables in hypothesized mediation model (N = 345).

3.2.2.1. Direct paths

Unstandardized path coefficients for the hypothesized model are shown in Figure 2. The total effect of pandemic trauma at Time 1 on boredom at Time 3 (path c; not depicted in Figure 2) was statistically significant and positive (B = 3.26, SE = 0.59, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [2.10, 4.43], β = 0.39), while statistically controlling for participants’ pre-existing stress, age, and gender. The direct effect of pandemic trauma at Time 1 on boredom at Time 3 (path c’; depicted in Figure 2) reduced but remained statistically significant and positive when the mediators were additionally included in the hypothesized model (B = 1.50, SE = 0.67, p = 0.025, 95% CI = [0.26, 2.89], β = 0.18). An increase in pandemic trauma at Time 1 uniquely predicted a 1.50-point increase in boredom at Time 3, as measured by the MSBS, over and above participants’ pre-existing stress, age, gender, and emotion dysregulation at Time 2.

As expected, participants’ pandemic trauma at Time 1 significantly predicted their lack of emotional clarity (B = 1.16, SE = 0.24, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.68, 1.63], β = 0.43), difficulties with engaging in goal-directed behaviors (B = 1.33, SE = 0.21, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.92, 1.73], β = 0.42), difficulties with impulse control (B = 1.32, SE = 0.25, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.84, 1.81], β = 0.47), nonacceptance of negative emotions (B = 1.06, SE = 0.23, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.63, 1.51], β = 0.36), and limited access to emotion regulation strategies (B = 1.27, SE = 0.21, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.86, 1.67], β = 0.42) at Time 2 (path a), while controlling for their pre-existing stress age, and gender. In other words, an increase in pandemic trauma at Time 1 uniquely predicted more than a 1.00-point increase on the DERS-18, across multiple facets of participants’ emotion dysregulation at Time 2. In turn, poor clarity of emotions at Time 2 (B = 0.90, SE = 0.20, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.50, 1.29], β = 0.29) and difficulties with concentrating and accomplishing goals when experiencing negative emotions at Time 2 (B = 0.59, SE = 0.20, p = 0.004, 95% CI = [0.20, 1.00], β = 0.23) significantly and positively predicted participants’ boredom at Time 3 (path b), over and above their pre-existing stress, age, gender, pandemic trauma at Time 1, and the other facets of emotion dysregulation at Time 2. A one-point increase in these two facets of emotion dysregulation at Time 2, as measured by the DERS-18, uniquely predicted respective increases of 0.90 and 0.59 points in boredom at Time 3, as measured by the MSBS. The effects of impulse control difficulties (B = −0.06, SE = 0.25, p = 0.800, 95% CI = [−0.56, 0.44], β = −0.02), nonacceptance of negative emotions (B = 0.03, SE = 0.20, p = 0.880, 95% CI = [−0.35, 0.44], β = 0.01), and limited access to emotion regulation strategies (B = −0.00, SE = 0.22, p = 0.990, 95% CI = [−0.39, 0.45], β = −0.00) at Time 2 on boredom at Time 3 were small and non-significant.

Though not depicted in Figure 2, participants’ pre-existing stress was positively related to their pandemic trauma at Time 1 (B = 0.22, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.13, 0.31], β = 0.34), difficulties with concentrating and accomplishing goals at Time 2 (B = 0.22, SE = 0.11, p = 0.045, 95% CI = [−0.01, 0.42], β = 0.11), nonacceptance of negative emotions at Time 2 (B = 0.30, SE = 0.11, p = 0.009, 95% CI = [0.06, 0.51], β = 0.16), and boredom at Time 3 (B = 0.69, SE = 0.30, p = 0.021, 95% CI = [0.10, 1.26], β = 0.13). Older participants (B = −0.02, SE = 0.00, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.03, −0.01], β = −0.26) and female participants (B = −0.04, SE = 0.02, p = 0.039, 95% CI = [−0.07, −0.00], β = −0.14) reported less pandemic trauma at Time 1. Older participants also endorsed less emotion dysregulation at Time 2, specifically less difficulties with emotional clarity (B = −0.03, SE = 0.01, p = 0.006, 95% CI = [−0.05, −0.01], β = −0.15), concentrating and accomplishing their goals (B = −0.05, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.07, −0.03], β = −0.23), controlling their impulses (B = −0.04, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.05, −0.02], β = −0.18), accepting their negative emotions (B = −0.03, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.05, −0.02], β = −0.17), and the belief in their emotion regulation abilities (B = −0.05, SE = 0.01, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [−0.07, −0.03], β = −0.24). Older participants additionally reported fewer burdensome pre-existing stressors (B = −3.83, SE = 1.42, p = 0.007, 95% CI = [−6.64, −0.99], β = −0.13). Finally, age (B = −0.05, SE = 0.03, p = 0.063, 95% CI = [−0.10, 0.00], β = −0.09) and gender (B = −0.06, SE = 0.09, p = 0.497, 95% CI = [−0.23, 0.11], β = −0.03) were unrelated to boredom at Time 3.

3.2.2.2. Indirect paths

Unstandardized estimates and the 95% CIs for the indirect paths are shown in Table 4. Pandemic trauma at Time 1 predicted an increase of 1.04 points in boredom at Time 3 (SE = 0.30, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [0.57, 1.76], β = 0.13) through its increased effects on participants’ lack of emotional clarity at Time 2. Similarly, pandemic trauma at Time 1 predicted an increase of 0.79 points in boredom at Time 3 (SE = 0.29, p = 0.006, 95% CI = [0.30, 1.46], β = 0.09) through its increased effects on participants’ difficulties with engaging in goal-directed behaviors at Time 2. None of the other indirect pathways were supported (Table 4). The total indirect effect was also significant (B = 1.78, SE = 0.37, p < 0.001, 95% CI = [1.16, 2.65], β = 0.21). Examination of the standardized indirect effects indicated medium effect sizes for the specific indirect effects of emotional clarity and goals, as well as a large effect size for the total indirect effect (see Table 4).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Unstandardized and standardized estimates and confidence intervals for indirect pathways to boredom (N = 344).

3.3. Exploratory analyses

The link between boredom proneness (BP) and the feeling of boredom has been well-established both prior to and during the pandemic (e.g., Mercer-Lynn et al., 2014; Martarelli et al., 2021), underscoring the relevance of BP to people’s experience of boredom. We estimated a post-hoc mediation model3, which would allow us to explore the predictive relevance of people’s pandemic trauma and emotion dysregulation on their boredom during the pandemic, while accounting for their BP. Participants’ BP was assessed at Time 1 using the six-item Trait Boredom Scale (Gorelik and Eastwood, 2019 under review, ω = 0.90). Supported by the BP literature reviewed in the introduction, paths from BP to boredom (at Time 3) and to emotion dysregulation (at Time 2) were added in the exploratory mediation model. Additionally, BP was set to covary with participants’ pre-existing stress, age, and gender. The exploratory mediation model demonstrated an acceptable-to-good fit with the data, χ2(34) = 136.924, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.953, RMSEA = 0.096, and SRMR = 0.072. The results for the a, b, and indirect paths, which are presented in Table 5, are consistent with the results observed in our hypothesized mediation model. The total effect of pandemic trauma at Time 1 on boredom at Time 3 (path c) was statistically significant and positive (B = 1.59, SE = 0.55, p = 0.004, 95% CI = [0.52, 2.64], β = 0.19), while controlling for participants’ BP, pre-existing stress, age, and gender. The direct effect of pandemic trauma at Time 1 on boredom at Time 3 (path c’) was no longer statistically significant when emotion dysregulation at Time 2, as measured by the five DERS subscales, was added to the model (B = 0.57, SE = 0.58, p = 0.320, 95% CI = [−0.50, 1.75], β = 0.07). Most notably, however, even when BP was included in the model, participants’ lack of emotional clarity (B = 0.56, SE = 0.23, p = 0.016, 95% CI = [0.22, 1.17], β = 0.07) and difficulties with engaging in goal-directed behaviors (B = 0.46, SE = 0.22, p = 0.034, 95% CI = [0.11, 0.98], β = 0.06) at Time 2 fully mediated the relationship between pandemic trauma at Time 1 and boredom at Time 3. Indeed, poor clarity of emotions at Time 2 (B = 0.68, SE = 0.20, p = 0.001, 95% CI = [0.30, 1.07], β = 0.22) and difficulties with concentrating and accomplishing goals when experiencing negative emotions at Time 2 (B = 0.46, SE = 0.19, p = 0.017, 95% CI = [0.08, 0.84], β = 0.18) significantly and positively predicted boredom at Time 3 (path b), over and above participants’ BP, pre-existing stress, age, gender, pandemic trauma at Time 1, and emotion dysregulation at Time 2. Taken together, our theorized mediation model is supported even when BP is modeled and controlled for.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Exploratory Mediation Analyses Controlling For Boredom Proneness (BP).

4. Discussion

The COVID-19 pandemic, which has thrown the world into turmoil for more than 2 years, has underscored the pressing need to understand the effect of the pandemic on people’s emotional wellbeing, particularly in light of the increasing probability of experiencing future pandemics as severe as COVID-19 (e.g., Marani et al., 2021). Shifting away from the emphasis on external pandemic-related factors, such as constraint and monotony, as well as the emphasis on individual differences (e.g., BP), the current longitudinal study sought to examine (a) if trauma predicts subsequent boredom and (b) if emotion dysregulation mediates this relationship. The current study took place during the height of the pandemic, between 6 May and 18 June, 2020, where COVID-19 cases and deaths increased substantially and unemployment rates were at unprecedented levels in the U.S. This provided us with the timely opportunity to examine these emotional contributors of boredom, an aversive feeling associated with a host of psychosocial difficulties and risky behaviors that jeopardize the effectiveness of containment measures that are designed to protect people from the otherwise wide-ranging traumatic effects of pandemics (e.g., infection, exposure, income loss, and social isolation).

4.1. Overview and discussion of findings

Consistent with the above-reviewed research that links traumatic and stressful life experiences to the boredom (e.g., Stretch et al., 1996a,b; Silove et al., 1997; Chaney and Blalock, 2006; Pfefferbaum et al., 2007; Schweitzer et al., 2018; Volkert et al., 2013), people with more pandemic trauma at Time 1 and people a greater number of burdensome pre-existing (pre-pandemic) stressors felt more bored at Time 3, while controlling for all other variables in the hypothesized mediation model. Most notably, emotion dysregulation at Time 2 partially mediated the positive predictive relationship between pandemic trauma at Time 1 and boredom at Time 3, with medium indirect effects observed. In particular, people with greater pandemic trauma subsequently felt more bored when they lacked clarity on their emotions and had difficulties concentrating and accomplishing their goals in the face of negative emotions. Moreover, our exploratory mediation model revealed that poor emotional clarity and difficulties with engaging in goal-directed behaviors (fully) mediated the relationship between pandemic trauma at Time 1 and boredom at Time 3 when BP was included in the model, thereby highlighting the unique importance of these two facets of emotion regulation on people’s feelings of boredom during the pandemic. We discuss these facets of emotion regulation further.

As defined by the authors of the original and short-form versions of the DERS (Gratz and Roemer, 2004; Victor and Klonsky, 2016), emotional clarity taps into the extent to which people know (are clear about) the emotions they are experiencing. Emotional clarity is related to the concept of emotional granularity, which refers to individual differences in the specificity of one’s emotional experiences or a person’s ability to make fine-grained, nuanced distinctions between similar emotional states (see Barrett, 2004), as well as to the concept of alexithymia, which refers to dispositional difficulties with identifying and describing one’s emotions (see Sifneos, 1973). Indeed, the lack of clarity (as measured by the DERS), low emotional granularity, and alexithymia are linked with a host of psychosocial difficulties—as observed both before and during the COVID-19 pandemic: depressive symptoms, anxiety, stress, PTSD symptoms, emotional eating, and avoidance of unwanted thoughts/feelings, to name a few (e.g., Barrett, 2004; Gratz and Roemer, 2004; Hallion et al., 2018; van Strien, 2018; Tang et al., 2020). Compared to the other DERS subscales, the clarity subscale has a significantly stronger negative association with indices of emotional expressivity (i.e., the extent to which people outwardly display their emotions; Gratz and Roemer, 2004) and mindfulness (i.e., acting with awareness and describing one’s inner experiences; Lavender et al., 2016). Within the context of trauma, empirical work has underscored the role of complex or multiple adverse life experiences on people’s inability to differentiate their emotional experiences (see Eichhorn et al., 2014; Zorzella et al., 2020 for a review). One study published during the COVID-19 pandemic found that a higher number of exposures to coronavirus was positively associated with more severe alexithymia symptoms among home-quarantined adults, including difficulties identifying and difficulties describing one’s emotions (Tang et al., 2020). Within the context of boredom, several studies, as described earlier, suggest a positive link between difficulties with labeling and describing one’s emotions and BP (Seib and Vodanovich, 1998; Gana et al., 2000; Harris, 2000; von Gemmingen et al., 2003; Eastwood et al., 2007). However, these studies present cross-sectional and correlational results among trait-based measures of emotional clarity and boredom, which precludes the ability to make conclusions about the temporal relations and excludes an examination of boredom as a state. The current study not only shows that the lack of emotional clarity predicts greater feelings of boredom at a later time point, but that the lack of emotional clarity is a mechanism through which people who experience more pandemic trauma subsequently feel more bored. We posit that those with greater pandemic trauma are subsequently unable to understand, articulate, and express their feelings, which may leave them “adrift”—lacking clarity on what is important and valuable to them. Taking together the above-reviewed definition and psychodynamic and existential theories of boredom, as well as the above-reviewed literature on the DERS, the inability to identify what one feels prevents one from being able to express and articulate their desires, which is a critical precondition for becoming engaged—thus, giving rise to boredom.

The goals subscale of the DERS assesses people’s ability to concentrate, follow through, and accomplish goal-directed tasks when experiencing negative emotions. This conceptualization emphasizes the ability for a person to behave in accordance with their desired goals when experiencing negative emotions (Linehan, 1993; Melnick and Hinshaw, 2000; as cited in Gratz and Roemer, 2004). As with the clarity subscale, difficulties with engaging in goal-directed actions is linked to a range of psychological problems, such as depression, anxiety, and stress (e.g., Hallion et al., 2018), and some studies suggest that the goals subscale is more strongly related to mood and anxiety-related symptoms, obsessive–compulsive difficulties, interpersonal sensitivity, somatization, and paranoid ideation than majority of the other DERS subscales (e.g., Coutinho et al., 2010). Compared to the other DERS subscales, the goals subscale is more robustly and negatively associated with indices of negative mood regulation (i.e., the belief that some behavior or thought will alleviate a negative state or induce a positive one; Gratz and Roemer, 2004), as well as more robustly and positively associated with dysregulated behaviors, specifically disordered eating and partner abuse (Gratz and Roemer, 2004; Cooper et al., 2014). Like emotional clarity, the ability to engage in goal-directed or desired actions is hindered in times of stress and trauma. Experimental work has demonstrated that in stressful scenarios, people’s engagement in goal-directed behaviors declines and, rather, their engagement in habits or routine behaviors increases (Neal et al., 2013). Neuroscientific findings corroborate this work, showing that elevations of stress hormones reduce people’s ability to make decisions and plan, but instead, increase their engagement in established routines (Schwabe et al., 2012). As described in the introduction, studies have found that BP is related to trait-based indices of diminished self-regulation and goal pursuit (i.e., “state orientation” and “Assessment” regulatory mode; Blunt and Pychyl, 1998; Struk et al., 2015; Mugon et al., 2018). The current study builds upon this correlational work, demonstrating that difficulties with engaging in desired actions—in the face of pandemic traumatic stressors—predict subsequent feelings of boredom. Drawing together the above-reviewed theories and correlational work, we posit that people with more pandemic trauma subsequently feel more bored partially through difficulties with engaging in goal-directed actions because they are more preoccupied with intrusive and persevering thoughts about these stressors or are rigorously evaluating different possible response options and outcomes; this impairs their initiation and maintenance of goal pursuit, leaving them unengaged. Future work could examine if the relationship between people’s difficulties with engaging in goal-directed behaviors and subsequent boredom involves or is independent from their state orientation and Assessment mode of self-regulation.

4.2. Implications and future directions

The current longitudinal study underscores noteworthy points about people’s emotional functioning and boredom during the height of the COVID-19 pandemic—during a period of increasing COVID-19 cases, deaths, and widespread unemployment. First, the results of the current study elucidate the role of pandemic trauma—consisting of people’s exposure to coronavirus and the financial, resource-related, and interpersonal stressors of the pandemic—on emotion regulation. People with greater pandemic trauma subsequently struggled to understand and accept their emotions, engage in desired goal-directed behaviors, and control undesired impulsive behaviors, and they lacked the belief in their ability to cope. These findings have the potential to be important, as they highlight the wide-ranging impact of pandemic trauma on people’s ability to regulate negative feelings engendered by such trauma—thereby highlighting the need for transdiagnostic evidence-based supports that facilitate people’s ability to emotionally cope with the pandemic (e.g., Waterschoot et al., 2022). Second, the results of the current study advance our understanding of boredom by highlighting under-researched, but highly relevant, predictors of boredom during pandemics: trauma and emotion dysregulation. The longitudinal nature of the current study emphasizes that people’s trauma related to the pandemic, as well as people’s lack of clarity on their emotions and difficulties with pursuing their goals in the face of such emotions, are risk factors for feeling bored. Given the above-noted associations between boredom and risky rule-breaking behaviors during the pandemic, as well as the link between boredom and psychosocial dysfunction, a fuller understanding of the predictors of boredom is a critical first step to informing the development of boredom interventions that can help mitigate the deleterious impact of boredom on people’s wellbeing—both within and beyond the context of pandemics. The current study’s findings suggest that interventions that focus on enhancing people’s ability to identify and describe their emotions, as well as enhancing their engagement in actions that are aligned with their values and goals in the face of negative affect, may be important next steps in this regard. For example, Acceptance and Commitment Therapy, which teaches clients skills to notice, describe, and manage painful feelings and thoughts, as well as to clarify and pursue value-based goals (Harris, 2009), may be a viable avenue for boredom-specific interventions.

The current study is the first to longitudinally illustrate that trauma and emotion dysregulation predict people’s boredom during the pandemic, as well as that emotion dysregulation partially mediates the prediction of people’s pandemic trauma on their subsequent boredom. When people’s BP is accounted for, emotion dysregulation fully mediates the prediction of people’s pandemic trauma on their boredom. In addition to advancing our theory of boredom and informing potential boredom interventions, this novel understanding of boredom introduces the opportunity to ask additional questions about the emotional factors that contribute to boredom. First, the current study used a longitudinal approach that underscores the temporal relations between pandemic trauma, emotion dysregulation, and boredom during the pandemic; however, we acknowledge that without an experimental design, our ability to make firm causal conclusions about the relationships between these variables is limited. Second, our mediation model controlled for pre-existing stress, age, and gender, thus emphasizing that when these factors are held constant, people’s pandemic trauma and emotion dysregulation uniquely predict boredom. Future research could seek to explore the moderating role of these factors, thus elucidating if and how pre-existing stress, age, and gender enhance or diminish the impact of people’s pandemic trauma and emotion dysregulation on their boredom. In the current study, we controlled for pre-existing stress, an important marker of psychopathology, such as adjustment disorder. However, we did not measure and examine the impact of pre-existing psychopathology on the paths estimated in our hypothesized mediation model. It is possible that different results (i.e., enhanced or diminished effects of pandemic trauma and emotion dysregulation) may have emerged if we included pre-existing psychopathology in our model. Thus, future work that explores the potential effect of this variable (i.e., as a covariate and/or moderator) on the generalizability of our findings is encouraged. Third, our work introduces questions about the moderating role of dispositional traits in the pathways between pandemic trauma, emotion dysregulation, and boredom. For example, one trait pertinent to the current mediation model is experiential avoidance, defined as the unwillingness to remain in contact with uncomfortable private events (e.g., thoughts, emotions, sensations, memories, urges) by avoiding or escaping these experiences (Hayes et al., 1996). Although experiential avoidance is conceptually similar to both distress tolerance and emotion dysregulation, evidence suggests that experiential avoidance is distinct from both of these constructs (for a review, see Boulanger et al., 2010) and predicts psychopathology above and beyond general psychological distress (e.g., Gratz et al., 2008). In keeping with this literature, it is plausible that pandemic trauma (at Time 1) may subsequently predict greater emotion dysregulation (at Time 2) among people who are more unwilling to remain in contact with their inner experiences. Fourth, the current study introduces the possibility that other relevant constructs may mediate the predictive relationship between pandemic trauma and boredom. Traumatic and adverse life events not only result in emotion dysregulation, but these experiences also shake one’s meaning in life, which otherwise help people cope (e.g., Larner and Blow, 2011) and buffer against the effects of stressful life experiences on emotional functioning (e.g., Halama, 2014). Longitudinal work suggests that life meaning predicts lower boredom propensity across time, and experimental work suggests that manipulating perceptions of life meaning significantly changes boredom, such that people with low life meaning feel significantly more bored than those with high life meaning (Fahlman et al., 2009). As Van Tilburg and Igou (2012) have showed, boredom is differentiated from anger, frustration, and sadness insofar as involves a perception that one’s situation is meaningless (see also van Tilburg and Igou, 2017; Chan et al., 2018). Thus, future directions might consider examining if disrupted meaning is an additional mechanism through which pandemic trauma predicts boredom. Finally, additional longer-term longitudinal studies are needed to understand the nature of the relationships between trauma, emotion dysregulation, and boredom beyond the time points in this study. These studies will be especially critical when examining long-term and residual effects of the pandemic.

5. Conclusion

Prior research suggests that external constraints and monotony imposed by the pandemic and individual differences in BP are contributors to boredom during the pandemic. The current longitudinal study builds on this important work, finding that trauma and difficulties with regulating emotions are contributors to boredom during the pandemic. People with greater pandemic trauma subsequently feel more bored when they lack clarity on their emotions and struggle to engage in goal-directed behaviors when experiencing negative emotions. These findings provide another way of understanding why boredom is prevalent during a pandemic. These findings also underscore specific areas of emotional functioning (i.e., emotional clarity and engagement in goals) that can be targeted to help reduce boredom during this and other taxing and stressful times, and, relatedly, to help mitigate the psychosocial difficulties associated with boredom and boredom-related non-adherence to critical lifesaving public health measures.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by York University Human Participants Review Committee (Certificate #: e2020-119). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

VB, AW, and JE: conceptualization and writing—review and editing. VB: data analysis and writing—original draft. JE: supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by grants to JE from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC; Grant #: 496399).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

1. ^https://osf.io/8pexf/

2. ^There were baseline (i.e., Time 1) differences between participants who completed all three time points (N = 345) and thus were included in the hypothesized mediation model and participants who did not complete all three times points (i.e., they completed only Time 1 or Time 1 and 2; N = 1,011). The latter group endorsed higher scores on three of the four pandemic trauma indicator variables (excluding resource-related stress), the five DERS-18 subscales, and the MSBS, as well as reported a greater number of burdensome pre-existing stressors and were younger in age (all p’s < 0.05). That the latter group struggled more with pandemic stressors and emotion dysregulation at the start of the study, as well as reported more burdensome pre-existing stressors, could be why they did not continue to participate after Time 1 or Time 2 in light of such taxing and challenging circumstances.

3. ^This exploratory mediation model was estimated during the review of this manuscript. We thank the Reviewers of this manuscript for their suggestion.

References

Bambrah, V., Wyman, A., Friedman, E., and Eastwood, J. D. (2022). Examining the longitudinal associations between adjustment disorder symptoms and boredom during COVID-19. Behav. Sci. 12:311. doi: 10.3390/bs12090311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barari, S., Caria, S., Davola, A., Falco, P., Fetzer, T., Fiorin, S., et al. (2020). Evaluating COVID-19 public health messaging in Italy: self-reported compliance and growing mental health concern. Europe PMC. Available at: https://europepmc.org/article/ppr/ppr130576

Google Scholar

Bargdill, R. W. (2000). The study of life boredom. J. Phenomenol. Psychol. 31, 188–219. doi: 10.1163/15691620051090979

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F. (2004). Feelings or words? Understanding the content in self-report ratings of experienced emotion. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 266–281. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bench, S. W., and Lench, H. C. (2019). Boredom as a seeking state: boredom prompts the pursuit of novel (even negative) experiences. Emotion 19, 242–254. doi: 10.1037/emo0000433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bieleke, M., Martarelli, C. S., and Wolff, W. (2021). If-then planning, self-control, and boredom as predictors of adherence to social distancing guidelines: evidence from a two-wave longitudinal study with a behavioral intervention. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02106-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blunt, A., and Pychyl, T. A. (1998). Volitional action and inaction in the lives of undergraduate students: state orientation, procrastination and proneness to boredom. Personal. Individ. Differ. 24, 837–846. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(98)00018-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boulanger, J., Hayes, S. C., and Pistorello, J. (2010). “Experiential avoidance as a functional contextual concept” in Emotion regulation and psychopathology. eds. A. Kring and D. Sloan (New York: Guilford Press), 107–136.

Google Scholar

Boylan, J., Seli, P., Scholer, A. A., and Danckert, J. (2021). Boredom in the COVID-19 pandemic: trait boredom proneness, the desire to act, and rule-breaking. Personal. Individ. Differ. 171:110387. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110387

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bridgland, V. M., Moeck, E. K., Green, D. M., Swain, T. L., Nayda, D. M., Matson, L. A., et al. (2021). Why the COVID-19 pandemic is a traumatic stressor. PLoS One 16:e0240146. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0240146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brosowsky, N. P., Van Tilburg, W., Scholer, A. A., Boylan, J., Seli, P., and Danckert, J. (2021). Boredom proneness, political orientation and adherence to social-distancing in the pandemic. Motiv. Emot. 45, 631–640. doi: 10.1007/s11031-021-09888-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Browne, M. W., and Cudeck, R. (1992). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociological Methods and Research. 21, 230–258. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canty, A., and Ripley, B. D. (2021). Boot: bootstrap R (S-plus) functions. R package version, Vol. 1, 3–28.

Google Scholar

Chan, C. S., van Tilburg, W. A., Igou, E. R., Poon, C. Y., Tam, K. Y., Wong, V. U., et al. (2018). Situational meaninglessness and state boredom: cross-sectional and experience-sampling findings. Motiv. Emot. 42, 555–565. doi: 10.1007/s11031-018-9693-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chaney, M. P., and Blalock, A. C. (2006). Boredom proneness, social connectedness, and sexual addiction among men who have sex with male internet users. J. Addict. Offender Couns. 26, 111–122. doi: 10.1002/j.2161-1874.2006.tb00012.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Conway, L. G., Woodard, S. R., and Zubrod, A. (2020). Social psychological measurements of covid-19: coronavirus perceived threat, government response, impacts, and experiences questionnaires. arXiv [Preprint]. 1–42.

Google Scholar

Cooke, J. E., Eirich, R., Racine, N., and Madigan, S. (2020). Prevalence of posttraumatic and general psychological stress during COVID-19: a rapid review and meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 292:113347. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, J. L., O’Shea, A. E., Atkinson, M. J., and Wade, T. D. (2014). Examination of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale and its relation to disordered eating in a young female sample. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 47, 630–639. doi: 10.1002/eat.22278

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Coutinho, J., Ribeiro, E., Ferreirinha, R., and Dias, P. (2010). The Portuguese version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale and its relationship with psychopathological symptoms. Arch. Clin. Psychiatry 37, 145–151. doi: 10.1037/t65316-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Danckert, J., Mugon, J., Struk, A., and Eastwood, J. D. (2018). “Boredom: what is it good for?” in The function of emotions: When and why emotions help us. ed. H. A. Lench (Cham, Switzerland: Springer International Publishing), 93–121.

Google Scholar

DiGiovanni, C., Conley, J., Chiu, D., and Zaborski, J. (2004). Factors influencing compliance with quarantine in Toronto during the 2003 SARS outbreak. Biosecur. Bioterror. 2, 265–272. doi: 10.1089/bsp.2004.2.265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drody, A. C., Hicks, L. J., and Danckert, J. (2022). Boredom proneness and rule-breaking: a persistent relation one year into the COVID-19 pandemic. Behav. Sci. 12:251. doi: 10.3390/bs12080251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Droit-Volet, S., Gil, S., Martinelli, N., Andant, N., Clinchamps, M., Parreira, L., et al. (2020). Time and Covid-19 stress in the lockdown situation: time free, “dying” of boredom and sadness. PLoS One 15:e0236465. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0236465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eastwood, J. D., and Bambrah, V. (2021). Self-focused but lacking self-insight: the relationship between boredom and self-consciousness. Paper presented at the 4th international interdisciplinary boredom conference, Madrid, Spain.

Google Scholar

Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., and Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A desire for desires: boredom and its relation to alexithymia. Personal. Individ. Differ. 42, 1035–1045. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.08.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ehl, D. (2021). 6 ways to fight coronavirus lockdown boredom. Available at: https://www.dw.com/en/6-ways-to-fight-coronavirus-lockdown-boredom/a-56394277

Google Scholar

Ehring, T., and Quack, D. (2010). Emotion regulation difficulties in trauma survivors: the role of trauma type and PTSD symptom severity. Behav. Ther. 41, 587–598. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2010.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eichhorn, S., Brähler, E., Franz, M., Friedrich, M., and Glaesmer, H. (2014). Traumatic experiences, alexithymia, and posttraumatic symptomatology: a cross-sectional population-based study in Germany. Eur. J. Psychotraumatol. 5:23870. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v5.23870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Einsle, F., Köllner, V., Dannemann, S., and Maercker, A. (2010). Development and validation of a self-report for the assessment of adjustment disorders. Psychol. Health Med. 15, 584–595. doi: 10.1080/13548506.2010.487107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elpidorou, A. (2018). The good of boredom. Philos. Psychol. 31, 323–351. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2017.1346240

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., and Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of life meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 28, 307–340. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2009.28.3.307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., and Eastwood, J. D. (2013). Development and validation of the multidimensional state boredom scale. Assessment 20, 68–85. doi: 10.1177/1073191111421303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farmer, R., and Sundberg, N. D. (1986). Boredom proneness: the development and correlates of a new scale. J. Pers. Assess. 50, 4–17. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5001_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fenichel, O. (1953). “On the psychology of boredom” in The collected papers of Otto Fenichel. ed. O. Fenichel, vol. 1 (New York, NY: W. W. Norton), 292–302.

Google Scholar

Fox, J., and Weisberg, S. (2019). An R companion to applied regression. 3rd edn. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

Google Scholar

Gana, K., Deletang, B., and Metais, L. (2000). Is boredom proneness associated with introspectiveness? Soc. Behav. Personal. Int. J. 28, 499–504. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2000.28.5.499

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glogowsky, U., Hansen, E., and Schächtele, S. (2021). How effective are social distancing policies? Evidence on the fight against COVID-19. PLoS One 16:e0257363. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0257363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gorelik, D., and Eastwood, J. D. (2019). Development of the Trait Boredom Scale.

Google Scholar

Gratz, K. L., and Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 26, 41–54. doi: 10.1023/b:joba.0000007455.08539.94

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gratz, K. L., Tull, M. T., and Gunderson, J. G. (2008). Preliminary data on the relationship between anxiety sensitivity and borderline personality disorder: the role of experiential avoidance. J. Psychiatr. Res. 42, 550–559. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenson, R. R. (1953). On boredom. J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc. 1, 7–21. doi: 10.1177/000306515300100102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halama, P. (2014). “Meaning in life and coping: sense of meaning as a buffer against stress,” in Meaning in positive and existential psychology. eds. A. Batthyany and P. Russo-Netzer (New York, NY: Springer).

Google Scholar

Hallion, L. S., Steinman, S. A., Tolin, D. F., and Diefenbach, G. J. (2018). Psychometric properties of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS) and its short forms in adults with emotional disorders. Front. Psychol. 9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 30, 576–598. doi: 10.1111/j.1559-1816.2000.tb02497.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harris, R. (2009). Act made simple: An easy-to-read primer on acceptance and commitment therapy. Oakland, CA: New Harbinger Publications, Inc.

Google Scholar

Havermans, R. C., Vancleef, L., Kalamatianos, A., and Nederkoorn, C. (2015). Eating and inflicting pain out of boredom. Appetite 85, 52–57. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2014.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., and Strosahl, K. (1996). Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: a functional dimensional approach to diagnosis and treatment. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 64, 1152–1168. doi: 10.1037/0022-006x.64.6.1152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, L., and Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling. 6, 1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johns Hopkins University Coronavirus Research Center (2020). COVID-19 United States cases by county. Coronavirus Resources Center. Available at: https://coronavirus.jhu.edu/us-map

Google Scholar

Kelley, K. (2007). Methods for the behavioral, educational, and social sciences: an package. Behav. Res. Methods 39, 979–984. doi: 10.3758/BF03192993

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, K., and Green, S. B. (2016). The problem with having two watches: assessment of fit when RMSEA and CFI disagree. Multivar. Behav. Res. 51, 220–239. doi: 10.1080/00273171.2015.1134306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larner, B., and Blow, A. (2011). A model of meaning-making coping and growth in combat veterans. Rev. Gen. Psychol. 15, 187–197. doi: 10.1037/a0024810

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavender, J. M., Tull, M. T., DiLillo, D., Messman-Moore, T., and Gratz, K. L. (2016). Development and validation of a state-based measure of emotion dysregulation. Assessment 24, 197–209. doi: 10.1177/1073191115601218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, Y., Luan, S., Li, Y., and Hertwig, R. (2021). Changing emotions in the covid-19 pandemic: a four-wave longitudinal study in the United States and China. Soc. Sci. Med. 285:114222. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Linehan, M. M. (1993). Cognitive–behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York, NY: The Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., and Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods 1, 130–149. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McDonald, R. P., and Ho, M.-H. R. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting structural equation analyses. Psychological Methods 7, 64–82. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.7.1.64

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maison, D., Jaworska, D., Adamczyk, D., and Affeltowicz, D. (2021). The challenges arising from the COVID-19 pandemic and the way people deal with them. A qualitative longitudinal study. PLoS One 16:e0258133. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0258133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marani, M., Katul, G. G., Pan, W. K., and Parolari, A. J. (2021). Intensity and frequency of extreme novel epidemics. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 118. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2105482118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martarelli, C. S., Wolff, W., and Bieleke, M. (2021). Bored by bothering? A cost-value approach to pandemic boredom. Hum. Soc. Sci. Commun. 8, 1–10. doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00894-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martinelli, N., Gil, S., Belletier, C., Chevalère, J., Dezecache, G., Huguet, P., et al. (2021). Time and emotion during lockdown and the COVID-19 epidemic: determinants of our experience of time? Front. Psychol. 11. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.616169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mekawi, Y., Watson-Singleton, N. N., Dixon, H. D., Fani, N., Michopoulos, V., and Powers, A. (2020). Validation of the difficulties with emotion regulation scale in a sample of trauma-exposed black women. J. Clin. Psychol. 77, 587–606. doi: 10.1002/jclp.23036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melnick, S. M., and Hinshaw, S. P. (2000). Emotion regulation and parenting in AD/HD and comparison boys: Linkages with social behaviors and peer preference. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 28, 73–86. doi: 10.1023/a:1005174102794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Bar, R. J., and Eastwood, J. D. (2014). Causes of boredom: the person, the situation, or both? Personal. Individ. Differ. 56, 122–126. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.034

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., Fahlman, S. A., and Eastwood, J. D. (2013a). The measurement of boredom: differences between existing self-report scales. Assessment 20, 585–596. doi: 10.1177/1073191111408229

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Hunter, J. A., and Eastwood, J. D. (2013b). Is trait boredom redundant? J. Soc. Clin. Psychol. 32, 897–916. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2013.32.8.897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mugon, J., Struk, A., and Danckert, J. (2018). A failure to launch: regulatory modes and boredom proneness. Front. Psychol. 9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neal, D. T., Wood, W., and Drolet, A. (2013). How do people adhere to goals when willpower is low? The profits (and pitfalls) of strong habits. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 104, 959–975. doi: 10.1037/a0032626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pek, J., and Flora, D. B. (2018). Reporting effect sizes in original psychological research: a discussion and tutorial. Psychol. Methods 23, 208–225. doi: 10.1037/met0000126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pfefferbaum, B., North, C. S., Pollio, D. E., Wallace, N. E., Smith, R., and Jeon-Slaughter, H. (2007). Focus groups with children after the world trade center attacks. J. Loss Trauma 12, 349–363. doi: 10.1080/15325020701296828

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

R Core Team (2022). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.

Google Scholar

Reynolds, D. L., Garay, J. R., Deamond, S. L., Moran, M. K., Gold, W., and Styra, R. (2008). Understanding, compliance and psychological impact of the SARS quarantine experience. Epidemiol. Infect. 136, 997–1007. doi: 10.1017/s0950268807009156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rosseel, Y. (2012). Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 48, 1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schwabe, L., Tegenthoff, M., Hoffken, O., and Wolf, O. T. (2012). Simultaneous glucocorticoid and noradrenergic activity disrupts the neural basis of goal-directed action in the human brain. J. Neurosci. 32, 10146–10155. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1304-12.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schweitzer, R. D., Vromans, L., Brough, M., Asic-Kobe, M., Correa-Velez, I., Murray, K., et al. (2018). Recently resettled refugee women-at-risk in Australia evidence high levels of psychiatric symptoms: individual, trauma and post-migration factors predict outcomes. BMC Med. 16:149. doi: 10.1186/s12916-018-1143-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seib, H. M., and Vodanovich, S. J. (1998). Cognitive correlates of boredom proneness: the role of private self-consciousness and absorption. J. Psychol. 132, 642–652. doi: 10.1080/00223989809599295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sifneos, P. E. (1973). The prevalence of alexithymic characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother. Psychosom. 22, 255–262. doi: 10.1159/000286529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silove, D., Sinnerbrink, I., Field, A., Manicavasagar, V., and Steel, Z. (1997). Anxiety, depression and PTSD in asylum-seekers: associations with pre-migration trauma and post-migration stressors. Br. J. Psychiatry 170, 351–357. doi: 10.1192/bjp.170.4.351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Steinberg, L., and Thissen, D. (2006). Using effect sizes for research reporting: examples using item response theory to analyze differential item functioning. Psychol. Methods 11, 402–415. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.11.4.402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stretch, R. H., Bliese, P. D., Marlowe, D. H., Wright, K. M., Knudson, K. H., and Hoover, C. H. (1996a). Psychological health of gulf war-era military personnel. Mil. Med. 161, 257–261. doi: 10.1093/milmed/161.5.257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stretch, R. H., Marlowe, D. H., Wright, K. M., Bliese, P. D., Knudson, K. H., and Hoover, C. H. (1996b). Post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms among gulf war veterans. Mil. Med. 161, 407–410. doi: 10.1093/milmed/161.7.407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Struk, A. A., Scholer, A. A., and Danckert, J. (2015). A self-regulatory approach to understanding boredom proneness. Cognit. Emot. 30, 1388–1401. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2015.1064363

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tam, K. Y., van Tilburg, W. A., and Chan, C. S. (2021). What is boredom proneness? A comparison of three characterizations. J. Pers. 89, 831–846. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tang, W., Hu, T., Yang, L., and Xu, J. (2020). The role of alexithymia in the mental health problems of home-quarantined university students during the COVID-19 pandemic in China. Personal. Individ. Differ. 165:110131. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2020a). The employment situation – April 2020. Available at: https://www.bls.gov/news.release/archives/empsit_05082020.pdf (accessed 1 December 2020).

Google Scholar

U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2020b). The employment situation – May 2020. Available at: https://www.bls.gov/news.release/archives/empsit_06052020.pdf (accessed 1 December 2020)

Google Scholar

van Strien, T. (2018). Causes of emotional eating and matched treatment of obesity. Curr. Diab. Rep. 18:35. doi: 10.1007/s11892-018-1000-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Tilburg, W. A., and Igou, E. R. (2012). On boredom: lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motiv. Emot. 36, 181–194. doi: 10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van Tilburg, W. A., and Igou, E. R. (2017). Boredom begs to differ: differentiation from other negative emotions. Emotion 17, 309–322. doi: 10.1037/emo0000233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Victor, S. E., and Klonsky, E. D. (2016). Validation of a brief version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS-18) in five samples. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 38, 582–589. doi: 10.1007/s10862-016-9547-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volkert, J., Randjbar, S., Moritz, S., and Jelinek, L. (2013). Risk recognition and sensation seeking in revictimization and posttraumatic stress disorder. Behav. Modif. 37, 39–61. doi: 10.1177/0145445512449647

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Gemmingen, M. J., Sullivan, B. F., and Pomerantz, A. M. (2003). Investigating the relationships between boredom proneness, paranoia, and self-consciousness. Personal. Individ. Differ. 34, 907–919. doi: 10.1016/s0191-8869(01)00219-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wangh, M. (1975). Boredom in psychoanalytic perspective. Sociol. Res. 42, 538–550.

Google Scholar

Waterschoot, J., Morbée, S., Vermote, B., Brenning, K., Flamant, N., Vansteenkiste, M., et al. (2022). Emotion regulation in times of COVID-19: a person-centered approach based on self-determination theory. Curr. Psychol., 1–15. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02623-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wessels, M., Utegaliyev, N., Bernhard, C., Welsch, R., Oberfeld, D., Thönes, S., et al. (2022). Adapting to the pandemic: longitudinal effects of social restrictions on time perception and boredom during the COVID-19 pandemic in Germany. Sci. Rep. 12:1863. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-05495-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Westgate, E. C., Buttrick, N., Lin, Y., El Helou, G., Agostini, M., Belanger, J., et al. (2022). Pandemic boredom: little evidence that lockdown-related boredom affects risky public health behaviors across 116 countries. arXiv [Preprint].

Google Scholar

White, A. (1998). Ho hum: a phenomenology of boredom. J. Soc. Exist. Analy. 9, 69–81.

Google Scholar

Wickham, H. (2016). Ggplot 2: Elegant graphics for data analysis. Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany: Springer Verlag.

Google Scholar

Wolff, W., Martarelli, C. S., Schüler, J., and Bieleke, M. (2020). High boredom proneness and low trait self-control impair adherence to social distancing guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 17, 1–10. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17155420

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, Q., Zheng, R., Fu, Y., Mu, Q., and Li, J. (2021). Mental health consequences during alerting situations and recovering to a new normal of coronavirus epidemic in 2019: a cross-sectional study based on the affected population. BMC Public Health 21:1499. doi: 10.1186/s12889-021-11550-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zorzella, K. P., Muller, R. T., Cribbie, R. A., Bambrah, V., and Classen, C. C. (2020). The role of alexithymia in trauma therapy outcomes: examining improvements in PTSD, dissociation, and interpersonal problems. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 12, 20–28. doi: 10.1037/tra0000433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Keywords: longitudinal, boredom, COVID-19, trauma, emotion dysregulation, emotional clarity, goals

Citation: Bambrah V, Wyman A and Eastwood JD (2023) A longitudinal approach to understanding boredom during pandemics: The predictive roles of trauma and emotion dysregulation. Front. Psychol. 13:1050073. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1050073

Received: 21 September 2022; Accepted: 09 December 2022;
Published: 13 January 2023.

Edited by:

David Anaki, Bar-Ilan University, Israel

Reviewed by:

Corinna S. Martarelli, Swiss Distance University Institute, Switzerland
McWelling Todman, The New School, United States

Copyright © 2023 Bambrah, Wyman and Eastwood. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Veerpal Bambrah, ✉ bambrahv@yorku.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.