Skip to main content
  • Research article
  • Open access
  • Published:

Improving the impact of non-pharmaceutical interventions during COVID-19: examining the factors that influence engagement and the impact on individuals

Abstract

Background

During an evolving outbreak or pandemic, non-pharmaceutical interventions (NPIs) including physical distancing, isolation, and mask use may flatten the peak in communities. However, these strategies rely on community understanding and motivation to engage to ensure appropriate compliance and impact. To support current activities for COVID-19, the objectives of this narrative review was to identify the key determinants impacting on engagement.

Methods

An integrative narrative literature review focused on NPIs. We aimed to identify published peer-reviewed articles that focused on the general community (excluding healthcare workers), NPIs (including school closure, quarantine, isolation, physical distancing and hygiene behaviours), and factors/characteristics (including social, physical, psychological, capacity, motivation, economic and demographic) that impact on engagement.

Results

The results revealed that there are a range of demographic, social and psychological factors underpinning engagement with quarantine, school closures, and personal protective behaviours. Aside from the factors impacting on acceptance and compliance, there are several key community concerns about their use that need to be addressed including the potential for economic consequences.

Conclusion

It is important that we acknowledge that these strategies will have an impact on an individual and the community. By understanding the barriers, we can identify what strategies need to be adopted to motivate individuals and improve community compliance. Using a behavioural framework to plan interventions based on these key barriers, will also ensure countries implement appropriate and targeted responses.

Peer Review reports

Background

Over the last 20 years a range of public health emergencies have resulted from the emergence or re-emergence of a pathogen. While the characteristics of each situation has been unique, there have also been underlying similarities. One of the main threads that runs through the history of these events is the need for community participation and engagement in the strategies proposed by governments. Since the first report about a novel strain of coronavirus, severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2) emerging in December 2019 [1], governments around the world has acted rapidly to introduce a range of public health measures (referred to as non-pharmaceutical interventions, NPIs) aimed at reducing contact rates in the population and thereby reducing transmission of the virus. These interventions include the measures that individuals and communities can take to slow the spread of infection especially during a period when vaccines and medical treatments are not available [2]. NPIs include compulsory measures underwritten by public health orders, such as closures of various services and establishments, quarantine/isolation and restrictions on movement; and voluntary measures, supported by health promotion, such as disinfection of hands and surfaces, mask use working from home and maintaining a physical distance from other. In some settings these measures are voluntary, whereas in others they are now enforced (such as, via fines and/or jail time). Alongside the use of these measures, there is the critical need to test all people with suspected infection as quickly as possible, to promptly isolate cases, and trace and quarantine their contacts.

Experience from prior events has underscored that considerable cooperation is needed from the public to successfully implement these NPIs during an outbreak or pandemic. However, it is not always easy to predict what the public reaction to an unfamiliar event could be. While pandemic and/or emergency planning guidelines aim to promote compliance with these strategies, badly judged communication approaches can lead to members of the community becoming “complacent, panicked, or otherwise acting in unhelpful ways” [3], setting back or interfering with the ultimate aim of the control efforts. Previous commentaries on pandemic plans suggest that documents have glossed over or failed to account for how members of the public may respond in an infectious disease event [3]. In addition many of these documents are based on the assumption that engagement with the public does not need to happen prior to (an event) but strictly once things start to occur and at that point facts (what is the disease/how is it transmitted) and instructions should be applied only [3]. It perhaps not surprising that these emergency control guidelines and plans lack sufficient recognition of the enablers and barriers to effective public responses [4].

It is essential that we understand the factors that impact engagement with these mitigation strategies, to facilitate realistic expectations amongst the community and to devise appropriate communication strategies to promote acceptance and uptake. A 2010 review paper by Bish and Michie identified several demographic and psychological determinants associated with a higher probability of adopting protective behaviours during a pandemic [5]. Building upon this work and to support current activities for COVID-19, the objectives of this review were to not only identify the key determinants impacting on engagement with individual protective behaviours and NPIs but to also explore the impact of these strategies on the individual. Lastly, we aimed to map the key issues identified using a behaviour change framework so that we could identify relevant interventions to support and enhance community participation.

Methods

Search strategy

This study involved an integrative narrative review focused on NPIs. These interventions (previously referred to as community mitigation measures) are a diverse group of measures that people, and communities can take to slow the spread of infection. They include personal protective measures (e.g., respiratory etiquette, face covering use and hand washing), quarantine of exposed community members and isolation of cases, measures aimed at increasing physical distancing (e.g., school closures and dismissals, redesign of living and working places, and postponing or cancelling mass gatherings); and environmental measures (e.g., routine cleaning of frequently touched surfaces) [6]. In the literature, they can also be grouped as either preventive or avoidance behaviours. When considering the adoption of these strategies, it is recognized that each community is unique, and that the selection will vary based on the level of community transmission, characteristics of the community and their populations, and the local capacity to implement strategies.

In this article, we aimed to explore published peer-reviewed articles that focused on the general community (excluding healthcare workers), strategies (both preventive and avoidance focused) and factors/characteristics (including social, physical, psychological, capacity, economic, motivation and demographic) that impact on the effective implementation. The topics that are covered in this review were identified by the researchers as being potentially relevant to government and to individuals and relevant at the time of writing. The review of each topic is not exhaustive but rather a reflection into the related issues impact of their use, as well as the potential strategies to promote engagement. Lastly, an extensive discourse into the ethical implications of these strategies has not been included in this review, as previous publications have covered this, and we did not want to do discredit to this important topic. In alignment with previous authors, we acknowledge that ethics (and values) are key factors that needs to be considered but outside the scope of the current paper. Lastly, this paper does not attempt to comment on the rational for the recommendations, nor the evidence of effectiveness for the NPIs being utilised during this COVID-19 pandemic.

Eligibility criteria

We searched publications in English on Medline, PubMed, Scopus and Google Scholar for the period between January 1, 2000 and to March 5, 2020, using a combination of search terms including ‘social or physical distancing’, ‘community mitigation’, ‘non-pharmaceutical interventions’, ‘community’, ‘general public’, ‘quarantine’, ‘social/university/childcare closure’, ‘hygiene’ and ‘pandemic or epidemic or individual disease names’. Additional, handsearching of articles bibliographies was undertaken. As this was a rapid review, we limited our screening to the first ten (10) pages for each term evaluated. At least two authors were responsible for identifying and reviewing extracted articles for each of the focus topics and for coming to a consensus on the findings included in this paper. The team of authors included a mix of academics and those working within local health departments so that we could ensure a mix of views and to support the extraction and adoption of relevant study findings. The team included members with experience of working in low, middle, and high-income countries. This review included quantitative (survey-based, observational studies) and qualitative (in-depth interviews, focus groups) studies which focused on the ‘community’ in general or focused on a discrete section of the community. We also included studies that were undertaken in response to the emergence of infectious disease events (SARS, MERS, and 2009 H1N1/A pandemic influenza) as well as studies published on COVID-19 (as of July 2020) and hypothetical pandemics (pre 2009).

Screening and analysis plan

A data extraction form was developed by HS and applied to the identified studies so that there was a level of consistency in the approach. The data extraction form included publication details, study methodology, setting and location, population targeted, and identified factors and recommendations from the authors. We performed a content analysis of all the data, summarized under the focus areas and then compared the findings. Using the behaviour change framework developed by Susan Michie and colleagues as an analytical lens for this review, we then applied the three essential conditions – capability, opportunity, and motivation (COM-B system) – to the identified issues to outline suggested intervention functions that could be implemented to address these deficits. The Behaviour Change Wheel (BCW) is a guide to designing interventions using a theoretical approach [7]. To help research to identify the target behaviour, the following steps are followed: (1) using behavioural terms define the problem, ensuring that one is specific about the target population and the behaviour; (2) from a list of potential competing behaviours select the target behaviour; (3) specify the target behaviour in terms of who needs to do what, when, where, how often and with whom; and then (4) identify the factor that needs to change in order to achieve the desired behaviour by analysing interview data using the Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) [8]. The TDF comprises 14 theoretical ‘domains’ representing a range of possible theory-based facilitators and barriers to behaviour change [9].

Results

Across each of the sections that were reviewed for this narrative paper, 53 papers were identified as being relevant and information was extracted using the screening tool. Given the rapid nature of the work and the ongoing developments with the COVID-19 pandemic, supplementary papers were included that have been published since the beginning of this pandemic. Not all papers are referenced into the results section, as some findings have been replicated and supported across numerous studies.

It is critical to firstly acknowledge the key role that psychological and social factors play in underpinning an individual’s willingness and actual adoption of behaviours or practices outlined in the following sections [5]. As these factors or combinations of factors can influence adoption of preventive and avoidance behaviours, we are going to dedicate this first section to reflecting on them. Factors such as how an individual perceives their susceptibility to the infection (rather than their actual risk), whether they believe the infection to be severe if acquired, their perceptions towards the efficacy of the mitigation strategy and their perceptions toward their ability to conduct the activity (self-efficacy), can all contribute to how an individual will engage with a preventive behaviour [10]. Further discussion about these factors is included in the sections below. Additional analysis can be found in the work of Bish and Michie [5], Catherine So-Kum Tang [11] and Joseph T.F. Lau [12] and others. Work published during this current pandemic continues to support the role that perceptions towards risk and efficacy have on the adoption of behaviour [13, 14]. The following sections will focus on the individual strategies or interventions that have been applied during this COVID-19 pandemic.

Personal protective measures

Mask use

In the studies reviewed, most authors have reported some level of ‘willingness’ to wear masks in the community, with higher levels of compliance (or willingness) reported amongst women, older adults (aged > 50 years), the highly educated (results are inconclusive in some studies), married individuals, and adults with poor self-rated health and/or home duties [15,16,17,18,19,20,21]. In regard to gender, it has been suggested that males may perceive the use of masks as being un- “manly”, and not use them due to variations in perceptions around health-related consequences [22]. However, not all studies have reported gender differences in relation to the use of masks, which may reflect the country of origin of the study (i.e., Australia and The Netherlands which do not have a culture of community mask use) [19, 23].

In 2014, Sim and colleagues published a review on the use of facemasks, drawing on the principles of the Health Belief Model [24]. They reported that individuals are more likely to wear facemasks due to the perceived susceptibility and perceived severity of being afflicted with life-threatening diseases. While perceived susceptibility was the most significant factor determining compliance, they also identified that perceived benefits of mask use did have significant effects on mask-wearing compliance as well. Focusing on the later, during the SARS outbreak, Tang et al. found that individuals were 1.4 times more likely to wear facemasks if they had strong beliefs in the effectiveness of wearing the products [21]. Beyond a belief in the efficacy of mask use, Lau et.al. reported that during the 2009 H1N1/A influenza pandemic, those who perceived that there was a ‘very high’ fatality rate associated with infection were more likely to wear facemasks regularly in public areas (OR 1.64, p < 0.01) [25]. Lastly, a lack of adequate knowledge about a disease may also hinder mask-wearing compliance. A survey from Taiwan identified that individuals were four times less likely to practice appropriate preventive behaviours, including mask-wearing if they lacked correct knowledge about the fatality rate of avian influenza [26]. Issues of access and affordability amongst vulnerable groups will also impact on mask use.

When it comes to cues to action, it has been suggested that corporate knowledge of outbreaks, social acceptance and perceived pressure from different sectors including employers, mass media, government and family can all play a role in influence compliance. Media blitz and public health promotion activities supported by government agencies provide cues to increase the public’s usage of facemasks. The survey conducted by Tang et al. (during SARS) reported that participants who were more aware of environmental cues (reminders from family members and/or government) were found to be 2.4 times more likely to wear facemasks [21]. In one study from Hong Kong, the authors reported that 95% of their survey participants believed that it was their “civic responsibility” to wear a mask in public places as frequently as possible and that they would wear a mask if they have any symptoms [18]. This is linked to the idea of social norms (or perceived social pressures) and highlights a difference between pre-pandemic practices between the East and West [11, 24].

Hand washing/hygiene

Hygiene promotion includes hand washing and sanitizing, respiratory etiquette and avoiding touching one’s face (mouth, eyes, or nose with unwashed hands). Studies have identified that compliance with hygiene strategies, especially hand washing, can be suboptimal and that the correct method may not be applied [27, 28]. Handwashing adherence depends upon complex behavioural considerations that are still being examined [29]. Much of the behaviour is automatic, habitual, cultural or determined by stimuli that are not open to conscious scrutiny [30]. For example, a mother might report that she washed her hands to avoid germs for the sake of offering a rational explanation, whilst her real motivation might be habitual i.e. the discomfort of sticky hands or their smell. Hygiene can be described as the need to be clean. If hands look dirty, then one is prompted to wash them (human emotion = disgust) [31] . The same can be said for cleaning a house (prompts: kitchen, toilet, surfaces look dirty) because it appears soiled. These actions may arise due to the presence of a disgust-evoking elicitor [32]. Beyond that, other factors that influence hand-hygiene behaviour include: (1) gender [27]; (2) habits which are acquired in childhood [33]; (3) social facilitation [34]; (4) modelling by people who may have perceived influence or social diffusers; and (5) environmental barriers, where lack of facilities or inconvenience prevent hand hygiene (affecting around 60% of the world’s population) [35].

Respiratory etiquette

The literature on this topic is far more limited, as studies tend to focus on hand hygiene and fail to comment on coughing, sneezing, and spitting [36]. One cross-sectional study conducted in Hong Kong during SARS found that being older, female, more educated and having higher risk perceptions and being anxious were linked to a greater chance of adopting precautionary measures, including covering their mouths [37]. In comparison, a study undertaken in Korea (post outbreak of MERS) [38], reported that only 50% of their surveyed participants (adults aged 20–69 years) understood what the term ‘respiratory hygiene’ meant and the mean score for self-reported compliance with respiratory hygiene/cough etiquette was 2.37 ± 0.42 out of a possible 4 [38]. In support of these findings is a notable study from New Zealand that actually recorded coughing and sneezing events during the 2009 influenza pandemic to monitor respiratory hygiene behaviours [36]. The authors found that around a quarter of the respiratory events (27.3%) were uncovered, and there was infrequent use of the responses recommended by health authorities (i.e., covering with a tissue or handkerchief at 3.4% and covering with elbow or arm at 1.3%). Whilst a small study, it does highlight that the message of covering with elbow or arm (in the absence of a tissue) was not having the desired behavioural impact in that context.

Isolation and quarantine

In the past, compliance with quarantine has been linked to certain life circumstances such as educational status, work-related and financial concerns, family needs and the behaviours of others [39]. Studies have repeatedly showed that women are more likely than men to comply with isolation or quarantine requirements [39]. Age has been reported to be linked to compliance, with younger adults less likely to be compliant (alongside healthcare workers) [19]. Lastly, being married has also been reported to be linked to higher levels of compliance with quarantine orders during an outbreak of avian influenza [17]. Beyond the demographic parameters, a range of attitudinal factors have been associated with compliance with quarantine including perceived susceptibility, perceived efficacy and trust in the authority have all been found to be linked. Interestingly, perceived severity was not a reported factor in one study but it should be noted that ‘proportionality’ plays an important role in public’s compliance to the restrictive measures so probably can’t be ruled out [40, 41].

Isolation and quarantine can involve not only physical confinement, but also cognitive, affective, and spiritual isolation due to the limitations in the interactions with, respectively, health workers, relatives, and religious leaders. One of the main concerns is about the impact on job security and compromises in income (as per the school closure). This concern has been reported in all income brackets but particularly of concern to those who earn <$30,000 per year, those who would not be paid if away from work (self-employed/casual staff members/those who are unable work from home), those who lived in urban areas, those aged 18–30, and those who have only graduation from high school. Beyond the concerns about income, were those related to the ability to function while in quarantine with reference to access to groceries, healthcare services (including refilling medications) etc. As highlighted in one study, a concern existed about not being able to provide evidence of ‘sick leave’ because of not being able to attend the doctor. The last line of concern was about the capacity to comply with quarantine orders in households with large numbers of inhabitants (presence of extended family and visitors) in limited space. This concern was raised in an Australian study focused on the risk of pandemic influenza in Aboriginal communities [42]. These findings are also relevant to other settings in which the family unit is not limited to children and parents. In the same study, concerns were raised about the impact on the ability to attend funerals and important family gatherings. Having allowances such as these may be detrimental for the greater good but beneficial for the community. During SARS, Canadian residents (from three regions) who participated in focus group sessions suggested that without reciprocal arrangements (social and material support), individuals may resort to breaking quarantine, effectively being “forced to spread the disease” [41]. Participants stressed that, to create an environment for compliance and to justify the use of restrictive measures, measures must be proportional to the risk that is perceived by the public.

It has previously been reported during the SARS outbreak and the 2014 Ebola outbreak, that those under quarantine reported fear, loneliness, boredom, and anger, alongside feeling worried about the effects of quarantine and contagion on family members and friends [43]. Loss of intimacy and social contact, culminating in physical and psychological isolation has been documented [44]. Further details about these factors is included in the review by Brooks et al. [45]. Limited access to external resources which would normally provide comfort such as books, music, and toiletries also resulted in difficulty. Social stigmatization and loss of anonymity have also been linked to hospital-based quarantine [46]. Factors that amplify these factors including the duration of quarantine, the fears of infections, having inadequate supplies, and unclear information and communication [45].

School closure (childcare, primary/secondary school)

Studies conducted during the H1N1/A 2009 influenza pandemic found that parents may lack the motivation or willingness to comply with measures because: (1) they may not believe that school closure will have any impact; (2) they may not understand the rationale for the action; (3) they may not feel that their children are at risk for the infection; and (4) they may not understand the requirements [5, 47]. Lack of awareness of the technical term for ‘school closure’ tended to be associated with “permitting out-of-home activities”, misunderstandings also led to some parents still sending children to school [48]. Students may also be unaware about the advice given to avoid social or physical contact was given to prevent themselves from infecting others [49].

Questions have been previously raised about the capacity to reduce community interaction between young people during a school closure period. Compensatory behaviour among school children has been referred to as the ‘compensation of contact’ (i.e. children continuing to meet outside of school settings) and is recognized to be a key determinant of the success of school closure interventions [50]. If the contact rates of students rise during school closure periods, then the impact of the strategy is reduced [50]. However, the task of keeping the contact patterns of children low can be challenging; for example, studies focused on out-of-home activities during periods of school closure for the 2009 H1N1 influenza pandemic, documented contact rates of 20.5% in Japan [51]; 34% [52] and 69% [53] in the USA; and 75% [54] in Australia. In the Australian survey, the 233 students reported more than 850 out-of-home activities over 7 days [54]. The Australian authors felt this was an underestimation. Attending sports games/ team practices and congregating in playgrounds were the most common activities reported. There were also stories of high school children gathering for a local inter-school dance, the same evening that school closures were enacted in one suburb of Australia [55]. Perhaps not surprisingly, younger children are more likely to leave the house during school closure [51].

Financial burden

Loss of income (economic strain) due to family members needing to take time off work to care for children was one of the key issues reported in previous outbreaks and pandemics [56, 57]. In some settings, parents may not have the ‘luxury’ of working from home, due to the types of employment. School closures would confront single-parent households with the challenge of balancing income generation and childcare [4]. The issue of job security also was a common thread in studies [58,59,60]. The subsequent impact on business and the wider economy (from parents being absent from the workforce) was also raised in the event of prolonged outbreaks/pandemics. However, these concerns were not echoed in all studies. For example, Effler et al. found that loss of wages and childcare issues were not very important in their survey of parents during the 2009 H1N1 influenza pandemic. This is surprising given that 50% of their parents reported missing work due to school closures and a third had to make special childcare arrangements [54].

Self-care

In regards to assistance with keeping children at home, a pre-pandemic survey of 1697 US adults reported that 64% would not need any assistance [61]. Of those who said they would need help, 50% would rely on family members, 11% on friends and neighbours, while 34% would look to outside agencies (including government agencies, church and community groups, or voluntary agencies). However, concerns have been raised about people being forced to make compromises such as relying on self-care (leaving a child in his own care or in the care of a sibling younger than the age of 13). Self-care has been found (in non-outbreak/pandemic situations) to expose children to three types of elevated risks: (1) injury; (2) suffering emotional or psychological harm; and (3) poor development because of poor choices of activities when in self-care [62]. The child may also have higher levels of anxiety because they are poorly equipped to care for themselves/others and to deal with daily life [62]. It could be theorized that children in self-care may also be more likely to engage in compensatory behaviours.

Impact on delivery of school-based resources

The impact of school closure on children’s nutritional health has also been highlighted by several studies [56, 63]. Closing schools in the event of a pandemic could leave children without access to these school-based nutrition programs. For example, in the US the National School Lunch Program (NSLP) and the School Breakfast Program (SBP) enable schools (US public and non-profit private schools and residential childcare institutions) to provide meals to children during the school day. Disrupted learning was also raised as a key issue during the 2009 H1N1/A influenza pandemic. While governments to date have been able to extend holiday periods in China and in other Asian cities to delay the return to school date, prolonged school closures may result in students having declining test scores. While home-school or distance-school has been suggested as ways to mitigate any impact, these strategies may not be feasible to all students if they lack access to computers/internet [63]. In Taiwan, elementary school teachers provided tutoring for children over the phone in 2009, while classes were suspended due to H1N1 influenza, to reduce the impact on the students. In addition, they also monitored the health of the students and encouraged them to comply with the recommendations including adherence with chemoprophylaxis. In that setting, the authors reported a high level of support from parents for the closure [64]. In the US survey previously mentioned, the vast majority would be willing to give school lessons at home, although 47% would need some assistance [61].

Mapping barriers and facilitators linked to capability, opportunity, and motivation

The COM-B model is adapted from the validated and well-known Behaviour Change wheel, now widely used in public health contexts as a method for designing behaviour change interventions. Drawing on this model, the barriers identified during this narrative review were mapped across the three elements of capability, opportunity, and motivation and suggested interventions outlined in Table 1.

Table 1 Identified factors impacting engagement with strategies and possible interventions to consider

Discussion

Willingness to adhere to NPIs may be influenced by the severity of illness observed in the community, relative to the need for income and the level of community, individual, and family disruption. Compliance reflects the interaction of a range of modifiable and nonmodifiable factors including the availability of resources, socioeconomic status, perceived consequences that could result, and their perceived level of personal and local community risk [65,66,67]. From the literature, a dominant theme that emerged is how to create an environment that supports and promotes adoption. Previously it has been suggested that we investigate the value of rewards or suasion rather than compulsion or punishment. To support this approach, the strategies that are implemented need to ensure that: (1) they are proportional to the threat; (2) compliant communities’ members are offered reciprocal arrangements; and (3) there is clear and transparent communication from key stakeholders.

Emotionally driven behaviours such as fear and threat also need to be considered. However, appealing to fear may be useful in some situations but not in others, as it may lead to defensive reactions [68]. Coupled with this, is the issue of optimism bias, which is a cognitive bias that causes someone to believe that they themselves are less likely to experience a negative event [69]. This bias is common and transcends gender, nationality, and age. Concerns about risk are often heightened during novel threats (especially for an infection that people have no physical awareness of) and in situations when people feel they have limited or no control. As acknowledged by Peter M. Sandman and Jody Lanard, it is common for people to feel anxious and potentially alarmed when faced with this unknown situation [70]. To get used to the new threat on the horizon, people may start acting like it is already present. For example, Sandman and Lanard postulated that a person may start wearing a mask on the subway as a form of emotional preparation for a feared future disease outbreak. This behaviour allows people to ‘emotionally rehearse’ the situation, as it is a functional response. It needs to be legitimized and requires acknowledgement that this is an unfamiliar space, while avoiding promotion of over-reactions. As suggested, “fearful people feel better, not worse, when their fear is legitimized; it is a relief” [70]. As suggested by Sandman, an unfamiliar situation should be made as familiar as possible, so that citizens begin to take the risks a little more for granted and that the outrage level will decrease [71]. A key issue that was raised by Bavel and colleagues was around the impact that continued exposure to negative framed messages can have on people [10]. As highlighted in their review, was the suggestion that the intense media focus on the number of people infected and dying may potentially sensitize people to neglect their own personal risk [10]. More work is needed around this issue and how message framing may impact.

Masks are currently being promoted as a means of source control i.e. reducing the risk of transmission by people who have been infected but are asymptomatic and contagious. The challenge with this, is that we do not currently understand whether members of the public fully understand this concept. In the echo chambers of social media, there are posts focused on ‘mask use for all’ and ‘wearing masks for others’, however we do not have a strong understanding of whether this sentiment transcends into the wider community. Social media misinformation and lack of well-designed education programs without community engagement can impact on compliance, acceptability and proper mask wearing. This issue merits further examination. Further issues which have been raised around the use of universal mask use during COVID-19 include what type of mask should be recommended (cloth mask or surgical mask), whether the use of masks will lead to panic buying or hoarding (and potentially supply chain issues/shortages for healthcare workers), whether children aged 2 years and over should be wearing masks (or face shields) and around how to optimize the usefulness of the products. To support and encourage uptake, it has been suggested that their use should be restricted to situations where they will confer a benefit. This includes times when a person is in enclosed spaces such as on public transport, at the shops or at places of worship, workplaces or at entertainment venues [72]. It could be theorized that by encouraging more targeted use of masks, this may assist with enhancing compliance. Community members may appreciate the heightened risk of exposure in situations where physical distancing is not feasible or where there is more chance of prolonged and close exposure to people and hence the rational for mask use. They may be able to tolerate shorter periods of masks use, despite concerns about the mask being uncomfortable. In communicating about the use of facemasks, efforts should be made to normalize the behaviour, to reduce the risk of discrimination or stigma targeted at those that are exempted from use (for physical or mental health reasons) and which continue to focus on the community being part of the solution. As suggested by Wang et al. public health messages to the public must include: the rational for wearing the mask, how to select the proper mask, how to dispose the mask and a reminder to continue with other prevention and control measures [73].

Strategies and interventions to promote hand hygiene are well studied in the healthcare setting, but when it comes to the promotion of community compliance, the literature is somewhat more limited. Finding interventions that successfully improve hand hygiene remains a challenge; studies conducted to date have failed to show any impact on hand hygiene behaviours between intervention and control groups [74]. Despite this, there has been key work around communication to promote hand washing/hygiene. One such study highlights that currently communication materials infrequently exhibit information consistent with theories of communication for behaviour change. Instead they rely on fear-based messaging, instead of using positive emotional cues or social norm appeals. In moving forward, it is suggested that hand washing/hygiene educational materials should adopt concepts from positive deviance [75]. In addition to hand hygiene, it is necessary to investigate strategies to promote cough etiquette. While there is agreeance that we should not be promoting people to sneeze or cough into their hands (unless they can directly wash them), the recommendation to cough or sneeze into ones elbow may not be a social norm in all countries and for some people is may not be habitual. Further mass media and the use of targeted social media campaigns may help to normalize this behaviour. Lastly, there has been some discussion around replacing one behaviour with another. For example, instead of touching your face, hold a pen or take a sip of water. If we are going to try and implement these sorts of messages, we must design these campaigns with the communities.

In situations where there are low levels of perceived risk, parents may lack the motivation to enforce their children to avoid social interactions. Compounding this are opportunity issues such as conflicting messages coming from schools and health departments or insufficient information coming to support parents to home-school. This is perhaps not surprising given the results from a literature review from Uscher-Pines et.al that reviewed pandemic policies and practices around school closures and found that there is limited literature on school practices to promote physical distancing, as well as limited incorporation of school practices to promote social distancing into state government guidance documents [76]. In future events, it is suggested that public health officials must clearly communicate with parents both why the intervention (school closure) is necessary, and what the benefits to the community will be. The same messages need to be delivered by the schools as they may be a trusted agent in some communities.

Some of the most important lessons about the use of quarantine come from its application in Liberia for the Ebola epidemic. As summarized in a paper by Umberto Pellecchia [77], one of the key issues was the lack of community engagement and low levels of ownership within the community in the decision making and implementation process. Ensuring that the communication about the situation (especially in the issue of school closure), is not simply subjugated by external agents. Use the local knowledge and practices of the community to create an environment of dialogue. He suggested that scheduled meetings or forums for awareness at neighbourhood or village levels could have greater impact than high- level meetings in which citizens only receive ‘echoes’ of information via mass media [77]. Lastly, he proposed that we need to work with local leaders (Principles, Religious leaders, Community leaders) as well as healthcare providers/public health practitioners, and communication specialists to enhance reaching local communities during times when their engagement with these strategies is needed. These local agents may be seen as more trustworthy and hence will be accepted as more credible and satisfactory [78].

For some of the issues identified in this review, a solution focused on supporting behaviour change is not straightforward. The key example here is the issue of job security and sick leave. The promotion of self-imposed home curfews assume that people have the luxury to take time off as they can access sick leave benefits or can work remotely. However, as reported in one US paper, 33.6 million US workers (esp. low wage and gig workers (i.e. food delivery people/ride share drivers) cannot access sick leave and so are faced with the dilemma of ‘seeing pay checks shrink, or going to work and creating health risks’. This is a reality that would be played out in many low- and middle-income countries as well. The consequence of this situation is that presenteeism — attending work while ill — among private-sector employees without paid sick days may extend the duration of the outbreak in that sector. While this issue needs to be addressed federally or within states (by the changing of laws), a short term solution would be to encourage businesses to implement company policy that protects the jobs of people who are required to take time off (for school closures or due to being isolated).

Having reliable and trustworthy information is key in this situation for citizens to act upon and slow down the spread of COVID-19. The challenge is that health literacy levels (or the skills needed to obtain, understand, and process) vary across communities. As an impact of lower health literacy, people are less likely to access services or to understand the issues related to their health. During a pandemic, this may equate to mistrust in the recommendations and/or misunderstandings about risk, and lower levels of uptake of recommended interventions, including testing. Complicating this picture, is that in an exploding market of COVID19 facts and fiction, simply knowing about the risks (i.e. functional health literacy) is insufficient; individuals also need to be able to critically assess the information. It is critical that we ensure that communication approaches are tailored so they meet the needs of the target communities, in terms of messages and dissemination strategies. We need to increasingly draw on the known principles of communication [79]. One example could be to utilize personal stories into our communication strategies to normalize our thinking about these strategies. Patient stories are often used to promote best practices around quality and safety in healthcare, the issue is that this thinking has not been applied in our communication around community mitigation strategies for COVID-19. We also need to think carefully about how we use objectiveness and empathy, solidarity, and altruism to garner engagement.

This review was intended to be a rapid review of the literature to support the current actions being implemented worldwide. It differs from previous studies in that goes beyond the psychological determinants that impact on compliance to also look at other know factors that either impact on engagement or arise because of participation. It can serve as a benchmark to see how knowledge and recommendations have changed because of new evidence emerging during the COVID-19 pandemic. However, it has several limitations which need to be acknowledged. Firstly, it is not a systematic review, it does not include all the published literature on a topic. The selection and inclusion of findings may have been subject to unconscious bias. However, our team of investigators included people from both academic and health departments which may have reduced this issue. We purposefully chose not to comment on the quality of the papers within this review and chose not to limit ourselves to past pandemics but to capture the literature focused on both hypothetical and real situations. Both qualitative and quantitative studies were included to garner a rich understanding of each NPI strategy. As part of our reflection on these issues, we have ensured that we have included suggested readings to support people’s understanding.

Conclusions

One of the key steps in this context is to ensure that we have community participation and co-design on the development of communication messages and practical materials to support those who are imposed upon to comply with these community strategies. It is also important to map the strategies or interventions to ensure that we are addressing the relevant key individual and contextual factors that have been identified in this study and others. Lastly, we need to ensure that we have a community voice in all the interventions that we plan. Working with the community to design new communication messages, community outreach initiatives and support documents will ensure that they have a higher level of authenticity and that they are received and adopted by those we need to effectively engage.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

NPIs:

Non-pharmaceutical interventions

References

  1. Guan WJ, Ni ZY, Hu Y, Liang WH, Ou CQ, He JX, Liu L, Shan H, Lei CL, Hui DSC, et al. Clinical Characteristics of Coronavirus Disease 2019 in China. N Engl J Med. 2020;382:1708–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Anderson RM, Heesterbeek H, Klinkenberg D, Hollingsworth TD. How will country-based mitigation measures influence the course of the COVID-19 epidemic? Lancet. 2020;395:931–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Davis M, Stephenson N, Flowers P. Compliant, complacent or panicked? Investigating the problematisation of the Australian general public in pandemic influenza control. Soc Sci Med. 2011;72(6):912–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Garoon JP, Duggan PS. Discourses of disease, discourses of disadvantage: a critical analysis of National Pandemic Influenza Preparedness Plans. Soc Sci Med. 2008;67(7):1133–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bish A, Michie S. Demographic and attitudinal determinants of protective behaviours during a pandemic: a review. Br J Health Psychol. 2010;15:797–824.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Qualls N, Levitt A, Kanade N, Wright-Jegede N, Dopson S, Biggerstaff M, Reed C, Uzicanin A. Community mitigation guidelines to prevent pandemic influenza — United States. MMWR Recomm Rep. 2017;66:1–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Michie S, van Stralen MM, West R. The behaviour change wheel: a new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implement Sci. 2011;6(1):42.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Francis JJ, O’Connor D, Curran J. Theories of behaviour change synthesised into a set of theoretical groupings: introducing a thematic series on the theoretical domains framework. Implement Sci. 2012;7(1):35.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Cane J, O’Connor D, Michie S. Validation of the theoretical domains framework for use in behaviour change and implementation research. Implement Sci. 2012;7(1):37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Bavel JJV, Baicker K, Boggio PS, Capraro V, Cichocka A, Cikara M, Crockett MJ, Crum AJ, Douglas KM, Druckman JN, et al. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4(5):460–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Tang CS-K, Wong C-Y. Psychosocial factors influencing the practice of preventive behaviors against the severe acute respiratory syndrome among older Chinese in Hong Kong. J Aging Health. 2005;17(4):490–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Lau JT, Kim JH, Tsui H, Griffiths S. Perceptions related to human avian influenza and their associations with anticipated psychological and behavioral responses at the onset of outbreak in the Hong Kong Chinese general population. Am J Infect Control. 2007;35(1):38–49.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Seale H, Heywood AE, Leask J, Sheel M, Thomas S, Durrheim DN, Bolsewicz K, Kaur R. COVID-19 is rapidly changing: examining public perceptions and behaviors in response to this evolving pandemic. PLoS One. 2020;15(6):e0235112.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Betsch C. How behavioural science data helps mitigate the COVID-19 crisis. Nat Hum Behav. 2020;4(5):438.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Marshall H, Ryan P, Roberton D, Street J, Watson M. Pandemic influenza and community preparedness. Am J Public Health. 2009;99(Suppl 2):S365–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Cowling BJ, Ng DM, Ip DK, Liao Q, Lam WW, Wu JT, Lau JT, Griffiths SM, Fielding R. Community psychological and behavioral responses through the first wave of the 2009 influenza a(H1N1) pandemic in Hong Kong. J Infect Dis. 2010;202:867–76.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Lau JTF, Kim JH, Tsui HY, Griffiths S. Anticipated and current preventive behaviors in response to an anticipated human-to-human H5N1 epidemic in the Hong Kong Chinese general population. BMC Infect Dis. 2007;7(1):18.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Lau JTF, Yang X, Tsui H, Kim JH. Monitoring community responses to the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong: from day 10 to day 62. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:864.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Barr M, Raphael B, Taylor M, Stevens G, Jorm L, Giffin M, Lujic S. Pandemic influenza in Australia: using telephone surveys to measure perceptions of threat and willingness to comply. BMC Infect Dis. 2008;8:117.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Leung GM, Quah S, Ho LM, Ho SY, Hedley AJ, Lee HP, Lam TH. A tale of two cities: community psychobehavioral surveillance and related impact on outbreak control in Hong Kong and Singapore during the severe acute respiratory syndrome epidemic. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol. 2004;25(12):1033–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Tang CS, Wong CY. Factors influencing the wearing of facemasks to prevent the severe acute respiratory syndrome among adult Chinese in Hong Kong. Prev Med. 2004;39(6):1187–93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Lee LY-k, Lam EP-w, Chan C-k, Chan S-y, Chiu M-k, Chong W-h, Chu K-w, Hon M-s, Kwan L-k, Tsang K-l, et al. Practice and technique of using face mask amongst adults in the community: a cross-sectional descriptive study. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):948.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Brug J, Aro AR, Oenema A, de Zwart O, Richardus JH, Bishop GD. SARS risk perception, knowledge, precautions, and information sources, the Netherlands. Emerg Infect Dis. 2004;10:1486–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Sim SW, Moey KSP, Tan NC. The use of facemasks to prevent respiratory infection: a literature review in the context of the health belief model. Singap Med J. 2014;55(3):160–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lau JTF, Griffiths S, Choi K-c, Lin C. Prevalence of preventive behaviors and associated factors during early phase of the H1N1 influenza epidemic. Am J Infect Control. 2010;38(5):374–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Kuo P-C, Huang J-H, Liu M-D. Avian influenza risk perception and preventive behavior among traditional market workers and shoppers in Taiwan: practical implications for prevention. PLoS One. 2011;6(9):e24157.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Guinan ME, McGuckin-Guinan M, Sevareid A. Who washes hands after using the bathroom? Am J Infect Control. 1997;25(5):424–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Fung ICH, Cairncross S. How often do you wash your hands? A review of studies of hand-washing practices in the community during and after the SARS outbreak in 2003. Int J Environ Health Res. 2007;17(3):161–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Whitby M, Pessoa-Silva CL, McLaws ML, Allegranzi B, Sax H, Larson E, Seto WH, Donaldson L, Pittet D. Behavioural considerations for hand hygiene practices: the basic building blocks. J Hosp Infect. 2007;65(1):1–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Jumaa PA. Hand hygiene: simple and complex. Int J Infect Dis. 2005;9(1):3–14.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Curtis V, Biran A. Dirt, disgust, and disease. Is hygiene in our genes? Perspect Biol Med. 2001;44(1):17–31.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Oaten M, Stevenson RJ, Case TI. Disgust as a disease-avoidance mechanism. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(2):303–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Whitby M, McLaws M-L, Ross MW. Why healthcare workers Don't wash their hands: a behavioral explanation. Infect Control Hospital Epidemiol. 2006;27(5):484–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Drankiewicz D, Dundes L. Handwashing among female college students. Am J Infect Control. 2003;31(2):67–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Scott B, Curtis V, Rabie T, Garbrah-Aidoo N. Health in our hands, but not in our heads: understanding hygiene motivation in Ghana. Health Policy Plan. 2007;22(4):225–33.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Barry T, Manning S, Lee MS, Eggleton R, Hampton S, Kaur J, Baker MG, Wilson N. Respiratory hygiene practices by the public during the 2009 influenza pandemic: an observational study. Influenza Other Respir Viruses. 2011;5(5):317–20.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Leung GM, Lam TH, Ho LM, Ho SY, Chan BHY, Wong IOL, Hedley AJ. The impact of community psychological responses on outbreak control for severe acute respiratory syndrome in Hong Kong. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003;57:857.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Choi JS, Kim KM. Predictors of respiratory hygiene/cough etiquette in a large community in Korea: a descriptive study. Am J Infect Control. 2016;44(11):e271–3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Moran KR, Del Valle SY. A meta-analysis of the association between gender and protective behaviors in response to respiratory epidemics and pandemics. PLoS One. 2016;11(10):e0164541.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Morrison LG, Yardley L. What infection control measures will people carry out to reduce transmission of pandemic influenza? A focus group study. BMC Public Health. 2009;9:258.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Smith MJ, Bensimon CM, Perez DF, Sahni SS, Upshur REG. Restrictive measures in an influenza pandemic: a qualitative study of public perspectives. C J Public Health. 2012;103(5):e348–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Massey PD, Pearce G, Taylor KA, Orcher L, Saggers S, Durrheim DN. Reducing the risk of pandemic influenza in Aboriginal communities. Rural Remote Health. 2009;9(3):1290.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Barbisch D, Koenig KL, Shih FY. Is there a Case for quarantine? Perspectives from SARS to Ebola. Disaster Med Public Health Preparedness. 2015;9(5):547–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Tansey CM, Louie M, Loeb M, Gold WL, Muller MP, de Jager J, Cameron JI, Tomlinson G, Mazzulli T, Walmsley SL, et al. One-year outcomes and health care utilization in survivors of severe acute respiratory syndrome. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167(12):1312–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Brooks SK, Webster RK, Smith LE, Woodland L, Wessely S, Greenberg N, Rubin GJ. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet. 2020;395(10227):912–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Johal SS. Psychosocial impacts of quarantine during disease outbreaks and interventions that may help to relieve strain. The New Zealand Med J (Online). 2009;122(1296):47–52.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Uchida M, Kaneko M, Kawa S. Role of household factors in parental attitudes to pandemic influenza-related school closure in Japan: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2014;14(1):1089.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Braunack-Mayer A, Tooher R, Collins JE, Street JM, Marshall H. Understanding the school community’s response to school closures during the H1N1 2009 influenza pandemic. BMC Public Health. 2013;13(1):344.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Miller JC, Danon L, O'Hagan JJ, Goldstein E, Lajous M, Lipsitch M. Student behavior during a school closure caused by pandemic influenza a/H1N1. PLoS One. 2010;5(5):e10425.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  50. Cauchemez S, Valleron AJ, Boelle PY, Flahault A, Ferguson NM. Estimating the impact of school closure on influenza transmission from sentinel data. Nature. 2008;452(7188):750–4.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Mizumoto K, Yamamoto T, Nishiura H. Contact behaviour of children and parental employment behaviour during school closures against the pandemic influenza a (H1N1-2009) in Japan. J Int Med Res. 2013;41(3):716–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Borse RH, Behravesh CB, Dumanovsky T, Zucker JR, Swerdlow D, Edelson P, Choe-Castillo J, Meltzer MI. Closing schools in response to the 2009 pandemic influenza a H1N1 virus in New York City: economic impact on households. Clin Infect Dis. 2011;52(Suppl 1):S168–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Gift TL, Palekar RS, Sodha SV, Kent CK, Fagan RP, Archer WR, Edelson PJ, Marchbanks T, Bhattarai A, Swerdlow D, et al. Household effects of school closure during pandemic (H1N1) 2009, Pennsylvania, USA. Emerg Infect Dis. 2010;16(8):1315–7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Effler PV, Carcione D, Giele C, Dowse GK, Goggin L, Mak DB. Household responses to pandemic (H1N1) 2009-related school closures, Perth, Western Australia. Emerg Infect Dis. 2010;16(2):205–11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. McVernon J, Mason K, Petrony S, Nathan P, LaMontagne AD, Bentley R, Fielding J, Studdert DM, Kavanagh A. Recommendations for and compliance with social restrictions during implementation of school closures in the early phase of the influenza a (H1N1) 2009 outbreak in Melbourne, Australia. BMC Infect Dis. 2011;11:257.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  56. Johnson AJ, Moore ZS, Edelson PJ, Kinnane L, Davies M, Shay DK, Balish A, McCarron M, Blanton L, Finelli L, et al. Household responses to school closure resulting from outbreak of influenza B, North Carolina. Emerg Infect Dis. 2008;14(7):1024–30.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. Baum NM, Jacobson PD, Goold SD. “Listen to the people”: public deliberation about social distancing measures in a pandemic. Am J Bioethics : AJOB. 2009;9(11):4–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Blendon RJ, Koonin LM, Benson JM, Cetron MS, Pollard WE, Mitchell EW, Weldon KJ, Herrmann MJ. Public response to community mitigation measures for pandemic influenza. Emerg Infect Dis. 2008;14(5):778–86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Blake KD, Blendon RJ, Viswanath K. Employment and compliance with pandemic influenza mitigation recommendations. Emerg Infect Dis. 2010;16(2):212–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Seale H, McLaws M-L, Heywood AE, Ward KF, Lowbridge CP, Van D, Gralton J, MacIntyre CR. The community's attitude towards swine flu and pandemic influenza. Med J Aust. 2009;191:267–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Robert JB, Lisa MK, John MB, Martin SC, William EP, Elizabeth WM, Kathleen JW, Melissa JH. Public response to community mitigation measures for pandemic influenza. Emerg Infect Dis J. 2008;14(5):778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Kerrebrock N, Lewit EM. Children in self-care. Futur Child. 1999;9(2):151–60.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  63. Berkman BE. Mitigating pandemic influenza: the ethics of implementing a school closure policy. J Public Health Mnagement Pract. 2008;14(4):372–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Chen WC, Huang AS, Chuang JH, Chiu CC, Kuo HS. Social and economic impact of school closure resulting from pandemic influenza a/H1N1. J Inf Secur. 2011;62(3):200–3.

    Google Scholar 

  65. Dombroski M, Fischhoff B, Fischbeck P. Predicting emergency evacuation and sheltering behavior: a structured analytical approach. Risk Anal. 2006;26(6):1675–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Desroches CM, Blendon RJ, Benson JM. Americans’ responses to the 2004 influenza vaccine shortage. Health Aff. 2005;24(3):822–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Taylor-Clark K, Blendon RJ, Zaslavsky A, Benson J. Confidence in crisis? Understanding trust in government and public attitudes toward mandatory state health powers. Biosecurity Bioterrorism. 2005;3(2):138–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Witte K, Allen M. A meta-analysis of fear appeals: implications for effective public health campaigns. Health Educ Behav. 2000;27(5):591–615.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Strunk DR, Lopez H, DeRubeis RJ. Depressive symptoms are associated with unrealistic negative predictions of future life events. Behav Res Ther. 2006;44(6):861–82.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Fear of Fear: The Role of Fear in Preparedness … and Why It Terrifies Officials [https://www.psandman.com/col/fear.htm]. Accessed 1 Mar 2020.

  71. Sandman PM. Responding to community outrage: strategies for effective risk communication. US; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  72. The independent scientific advisory Group for Emergencies. Policy for England on face coverings to reduce tranmission of SARS-CoV-2. England: An Independent SAGE Report following public consultation on 14th July 2020; 2020.

    Google Scholar 

  73. Wang J, Pan L, Tang S, Ji JS, Shi X. Mask use during COVID-19: a risk adjusted strategy. Environ Pollut. 2020;266:115099.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  74. Warren-Gash C, Fragaszy E, Hayward AC. Hand hygiene to reduce community transmission of influenza and acute respiratory tract infection: a systematic review. Influenza Other Respir Viruses. 2013;7(5):738–49.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Quilliam ET, McKay BA, Lapinski MK, Viken G, Plasencia J, Wang Z, Fraser A. A content analysis of hand hygiene materials targeting elementary-age children. Health Educ Res. 2018;33(6):481–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Uscher-Pines L, Schwartz HL, Ahmed F, Zheteyeva Y, Meza E, Baker G, Uzicanin A. School practices to promote social distancing in K-12 schools: review of influenza pandemic policies and practices. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):406.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Umberto P. Quarantine and its malcontents. Anthropol Action. 2017;24(2):15–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Vaughan E, Tinker T. Effective health risk communication about pandemic influenza for vulnerable populations. Am J Public Health. 2009;99:S324–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Akl EA, Oxman AD, Herrin J, Vist GE, Terrenato I, Sperati F, Costiniuk C, Blank D, Schünemann H. Framing of health information messages. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2011;12:CD006777.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

There was no funding associated with this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

HS conceived and designed this study, assisted with the literature review, and developed the manuscript. CD, IA, KR, YS, MQ, ADD, JS, SI assisted with the review of the literature, interpretation, and review of the manuscript. All authors have read and approved the manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Holly Seale.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

Dr. Holly Seale is a Section Editor for BMC Infectious Diseases. The other authors do not have anything to declare.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Seale, H., Dyer, C.E.F., Abdi, I. et al. Improving the impact of non-pharmaceutical interventions during COVID-19: examining the factors that influence engagement and the impact on individuals. BMC Infect Dis 20, 607 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12879-020-05340-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12879-020-05340-9

Keywords