Skip to main content

The effect of cognition and affect on preventive behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional study in China

Abstract

Background

The global outbreak of COVID-19 has become an international public health crisis. Specific antiviral treatments for COVID-19 are not yet available, and prevention is of particular importance to fight the virus. This study tends to explore and compare the roles of cognitive and affective factors in predicting preventive behavior adoption during the COVID-19 pandemic in China.

Methods

An online survey using a quota sampling method to collect responses from 3000 Chinese adults was conducted from March 2, 2020 to March 23, 2020. Questions included sociodemographic features, coronavirus knowledge, negative emotion, risk perception, and behavioral responses. Multiple regression analyses were conducted to examine the predictors of behavioral responses toward COVID-19.

Results

On average, respondents had low levels of knowledge about COVID-19 (the overall correct response rate was 7.5%). Most respondents reported moderate to strong negative emotions towards the virus (3.47 out of 5). The average reported perceived chance of infection was 23.89%. For behavioral responses, respondents reported low frequencies of going out for activities (1.98 out of 4) and high frequencies of taking preventive measures (3.22 out of 4). Behavioral responses toward COVID-19 were found to be determined by cognitive and affective variables. Knowledge was negatively related to frequency of going out for activities (β = − 0.11, p < .001). Negative emotion (β = 0.34, p < .001), and risk perception (β = 0.05, p = .007) were positively associated with going out for activities. The explanatory power of affective variables (ΔR2 = 12.1%) was greater than cognitive variables (ΔR2 = 1.0%). For preventive behaviors, knowledge was positively associated with preventive behaviors (β = 0.22, p < .001). Negative emotion (β = − 0.28, p < .001) and risk perception (β = − 0.05, p = .002) were all negatively associated with preventive measures. Affective variables still showed stronger explanatory power (ΔR2 = 8%) than cognitive variables (ΔR2 = 4.4%) in predicting preventive behaviors.

Conclusions

After the rising period of the COVID-19 outbreak in mainland China, cognitive and affective variables still played important roles in predicting behavioral responses. Compared with cognitive factors, affective factors demonstrated stronger explanatory power in predicting behavioral responses toward COVID-19. The findings may have implications for enhancing individual compliance with guidelines of adopting preventive behaviors in response to COVID-19.

Peer Review reports

Background

COVID-19 poses a major challenge to global public health and prevention is of particular importance to overcome the crisis. The officially recommended preventive behaviors, including washing hands frequently with water and soap, wearing a face mask, social distancing and so on, are found to be effective in preventing the spread of the virus [1, 2]. However, there are considerable variation in terms of prevention behaviors for people of different characteristics [3, 4]. Understanding public health behavioral responses toward COVID-19 is vital for combating the virus. Therefore, it is important to explore the antecedents to desirable health behaviors during this global pandemic.

Previous studies have proposed various behavioral models to explain preventive behaviors at the individual level: the Theory of Reasoned Action, the Health Belief Model, the Social Cognitive Model, the extended parallel process model (EPPM), etc. Based on these models, researchers identified a host of factors leading to effective preventive behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic: risk perception, self-efficacy, negative emotion such as fear and sadness, knowledge, belief in conspiracy, socioeconomic status, etc. [5,6,7,8,9]. At the early stage of COVID-19, a number of studies have been conducted to identify which factors predicted preventive behaviors in China. Knowledge and concerns about COVID-19, optimistic attitudes, perceived severity of the disease, political trust, and support for the government and health authorities were found to be significantly associated with a higher likelihood of taking preventive measures for COVID-19 [10,11,12]. These factors can be roughly divided into two categories: cognition and affect. However, few studies have juxtaposed cognitive and affective variables in one study. Besides, most of the existing studies were conducted during the initial stage of COVID-19 outbreak. After the rising phase of the virus outbreak, the decreasing number of confirmed cases and mortality influenced people’s estimation about the severity and controllability of COVID-19 [13], and therefore the determinants of preventive behaviors might present a different picture.

Thus in the current study we ask the following two questions: (1) What are the roles of cognitive and affective variables in predicting behavioral responses toward COVID-19 after the rising phase of COVID-19 in China? (2) Which factor (cognition vs. affect) possesses greater explanatory power in predicting people’s behavioral responses? In this study, we used data from a cross-sectional survey fielded after the rising phase of the virus outbreak in China to further our understanding of preventive measures in response to COVID-19 in China.

Methods

Study design, procedure and participants

The data used in this study was collected between March 2 and March 23, 2020. Before March, the quickly spread of COVID-19 in China had attracted lots of research attention, leading to numerous studies concerning the causes and consequences of COVID-19 prevention [14,15,16]. In March 2020, the daily confirmed cases of COVID-19 dropped to single digits in China, thus we chose March 2 as the starting date of data collection because we believed that the situation could be different from that during the early stage of COVID-19. The end date of the survey was determined by the speed of data collection. To ensure survey data quality and to enhance response rate, respondents were invited for multiple times, and it took about three weeks to reach the target sample size.

Data for this study were obtained from an online survey of nationally representative internet users in China. The data collection was contracted out to a commercial marketing research company in China. The company maintains a large panel pool of 4 million participants covering all Chinese provinces. In order to achieve a representative sample, the study employs a stratified quota sampling technique to recruit respondents. The quotas for subcategories of gender, age and education groups are according to the most recent CNNIC (China Internet Network Information Center) report [17]. A total of 3000 respondents of 18 years’ old or above filled out the survey with a response rate of 24.56%.

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Fudan University. All the methods in the study were conducted according to the criteria set by the ethics committee of Fudan University and each subject signed an informed consent before participating to the study.

Measures

Knowledge of COVID-19

Four questions were designed from popular misinformation regarding COVID-19 in China. Previous studies measured COVID-19 knowledge using questions related to the clinical presentations, transmission routs, prevention and control, etc. Owing to the overwhelming number of news reports on this public health emergency, prior studies found that most people have known basic information regarding COVID-19 [18, 19]. Since knowledgeable individuals are less likely to be misguided by misinformation, a better way of distinguishing people with high levels of knowledge from the rest is to measure their ability to identify misinformation. We asked respondents to indicate whether they believed the statement we presented regarding COVID-19 on a 4-point scale, ranging from ‘definitely false’ to ‘definitely true’. When a respondent chose ‘definitely true’ or ‘true’, they received 1 point if the statement is correct, and 0 point if the statement is wrong. We summed the points of all four questions to formulate an index for knowledge of COVID-19, ranging from 0 to 4.

Risk perception

Respondents were asked to estimate their own risk of COVID-19 infection, other people’s risk of infection, as well as risk of dying from the virus on a 0–100 scale, with 100 being the highest perceived risk. We used the average score of the three indicators to operationalize risk perception.

Negative emotion

Negative emotion was measured by asking respondents to report their levels of ‘sadness’, ‘fear’, ‘anger’ and ‘shock’ in response to COVID-19 on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). The items were averaged for each respondent to formulate an indicator of negative emotion.

Going out for activities

Respondents were asked to report the frequency of going out for work, entertainment, for daily life necessities, and for social functions since the Wuhan lockdown on January 23, 2020 on a 4-point scale ranging from 1 to 4 where 1 means never and 4 means frequently. Respondents’ answers were averaged to form a score where higher score indicates higher frequency.

Preventive behaviors

Using the same 4-point scale, respondents were asked to report how frequently they had taken the following measures recommended by the WHO to protect themselves from COVID-19: wearing masks, washing hands with water and soap, avoiding hand shaking or hugging, avoiding eating in restaurants. Their answers were averaged to form an index where higher scores represent higher levels of preventive behavior.

Demographics

Respondents were also asked about their age, gender, education attainment, marital status, living area, and household monthly income.

For details of the measures, please see Table 1.

Table 1 Measure and range score of main variables in this study

Data analysis

All statistical analyses were performed using SPSS statistical software, version 26. Foremost, we performed the descriptive analyses to present the means and the Cronbach’s Alphas of the variables. Then, multiple regressions were conducted to examine and to compare the predictors of behavioral responses toward COVID-19. First, we only entered demographic factors into the model to examine how much variance in behavioral responses can be explained by the control variables. Then we added cognitive and affective variables separately into the model to compare the explanatory power of these variables. Last, we entered all the predictors simultaneously into the model.

Results

Sample characteristics

Of the total respondents (N = 3000), 47.6% were female, and more than 60% of them aged below 40 years. About 38.1% of the respondents completed secondary school and 75.3% of the respondents were married. In addition, nearly half had a monthly average household income between 10,001 and 30,000 yuan (USD 1414 to 4240). The majority of the respondents lived in urban areas (60.8%). The distribution of demographic variables (age, gender, education and income) of our sample are very close to those reported in the latest CNNIC report.

Descriptive analysis

According to Table 2, respondents had low levels of COVID-19 knowledge (M = 2.20, SD = 0.97, Min = 0, Max = 4). Only 7.5% of respondents (n = 225) correctly answered all the questions. For emotional reactions, most of the respondents had moderate to strong negative emotions towards the virus (3.47 out of 5). As for risk perception, people estimated that the probability of infection for themselves (M = 19.34, SD = 24.53) was lower than the probability of infection for other people (M = 29.89, SD = 25.27). The average estimated death rate for the infected was 22.43% (SD = 25.05), which is much higher than the actual figure in reality. For behavioral responses, respondents reported low frequencies of going out for activities (1.98 out of 4). Compared with going out for social functions (M = 1.77, SD = 1.02) and entertainment activities (M = 1.71, SD = 0.93), respondents went out for daily necessities (M = 2.34, SD = 0.80) and work (M = 2.10, SD = 0.97) more frequently. Meanwhile, results showed that the frequency of taking preventive measures was high (3.22 out of 4). Wearing a face mask had the highest frequency (M = 3.28, SD = 0.89), followed by avoiding hand shaking or hugging (M = 3.26, SD = 0.97), avoiding eating in restaurants (M = 3.22, SD = 1.00) and washing hands with water and soap (M = 3.13, SD = 0.91).

Table 2 Descriptive statistics and correlation coefficient (N = 3000)

The Pearson correlation analysis shows that: (a) knowledge is negatively associated with going out (p < .01) and positively associated with preventive behaviors (p < .01); (b) negative emotion is positively associated with going out (p < .01) and negatively associated with preventive behaviors (p < .01); and (c) there is a positive correlation between perceived risk (p < .01) and going out. But the relationship between perceived risk and preventive behaviors is negative (p < .01).

Predicting behavioral responses towards COVID-19

Multiple regression analyses were performed to examine factors predicting behavioral response towards COVID-19. For going out for activities (Table 3), the explanatory power of the demographic factors was fairly small (3.70%). Introducing cognitive factor into the model explained only an additional 1.00% of the variance in frequency of going out for activities (F (7,2992) = 21.47, p < .001). As Model 2 shows, knowledge was negatively related to the frequency of going out for activities (β = − 0.10, p < .001). Model 3 demonstrates that negative emotion (β = 0.34, p < .001) and risk perception (β = 0.04, p = .01) were also significant predictors of frequency of going out for activities. After adding the affective variables, the R2 increased from 3.70 to 15.90 (F (8, 2991) =70.50, p < .001).

Table 3 Predicting going out for activities during COVID-19 pandemic

These results showed that cognitive and affective variables were important explanatory variables, but affective variables had stronger explanatory power in predicting frequency of going out for activities.

We then entered all cognitive and affective variables simultaneously into the model (Model 4). Results from Model 4 were consistent with those from Model 2 and 3.

We performed the same set of analyses for predicting preventive behaviors. As was shown in Table 4, demographic variables only explained about 3.70% of the variance in preventive behavior adoption (Model 5). Cognitive predictors explained 4.40% of the variance (F (7, 2992) =37.89, p < .001) and the two affective variables explained 8% of the variance (F (8, 2991) =49.70, p < .001). The explanatory power of the affective variables was greater than that of the cognitive variable.

Table 4 Predicting preventive behaviors during COVID-19 pandemic

Respondents with higher COVID-19 knowledge were more likely to adopt preventive measures (β = 0.22, p < .001). Negative emotion (β = − 0.28, p < .001) and risk perception (β = − 0.05, p = .002) were negatively associated with preventive behavior adoption (Model 8).

Finally, our results find that behavioral responses were also influenced by respondents’ demographic characteristics. Respondents who were male (β = − 0.09, p < .001) and having low levels of household income (β = − 0.04, p = 0.04) were more likely to go out for activities. Respondents who were the elderly (β = 0.06, p = .005), female (β = 0.08, p < .001), with higher income (β = 0.07, p < .001) and married (β = 0.11, p < .001) were more likely to adopt preventive behaviors.

Discussion

Principal results

The aim of this study was to investigate the role of cognitive and affective factors in predicting behavioral responses among adult Chinese after the initial rising phase of the virus outbreak. Overall, our study found that both cognitive and affective factors were both significant predictors of going out activities and preventive behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Similar to previous studies, this study demonstrated that knowledge was a critical explanatory factor of behavioral responses toward the epidemic disease. The more knowledgeable the respondent is, he or she will be more likely to adopt preventive measures and less likely to go out for activities. During the very early stage of the epidemic, studies showed that knowledge was significantly associated with a lower likelihood of potentially dangerous practices in response to COVID-19 in China [20]. This is consistent with our findings, which highlight the importance of knowledge. It is worth mentioning that the overall correct response rate of the knowledge questions in our study is only 7.5%, which is much lower than those in previous studies conducted at the early phase of COVID-19. Such a gap is primarily because of the design of our knowledge measures. Adapted from widespread misinformation about the virus, these questions are a little tricky for people to judge. In addition, most of the previous studies conducted online collected data using non-probability sampling method, which might not reflect the entire Chinese online population. For instance, an overall correct rate of 90% on the COVID-19 knowledge was based on a sample with highly educational attainment (82.4% of the respondents held an associate’s degree or higher) [20]. In our study, the sample characteristics of the young, low education, and low income could be another possible explanation.

This study also found that affective factors played important roles in predicting behavioral outcomes. The more negative emotions an individual experienced, the more likely they were to go out for different activities and the less likely they were to take preventive measures. This is inconsistent with the findings from recent studies showing that negative emotion, exaggerated levels of anxiety and fear of COVID-19 in particular, was associated with positive behavioral change such as social distancing and improved hand hygiene [21]. Previous studies found that different patterns of emotional responses resulted in different coping strategies toward problems. Fear was linked with desire for protection but sadness was more likely to be associated with non-active coping methods [22]. By this logic, it is possible that our negative emotion measure was dominated by sadness more than fear. According to our data, the most common emotion respondents reported that they experienced were shock (3.70 out of 5) and sadness (3.47 out of 5). Another explanation could be the reverse causal relationship between emotion and preventive behaviors. During the rapid rise period of the COVID-19 outbreak, personal prevention measures were found to be associated with lower levels of stress, anxiety and depression in China [15]. In our study, the pattern is the same. The more adoption of preventive measures, the lower levels of negative emotion one would experience (b = − 0.09, p = .006). In turn, after the rising phase of the virus outbreak, low levels of negative emotion could lead to more preventive behaviors.

Perceived risk was also found to be negatively associated with the frequency of adopting preventive behaviors. Previous studies identified three hypotheses between risk perception and health behaviors. First, the accuracy hypothesis asserts that perception of risk at any time reflects one’s risk behaviors at that time. Second, the behavior motivation hypothesis argues that risk perception at one time leads to increased preventive behaviors at a later time. Third, the risk reappraisal hypothesis indicates that if an action is believed to reduce risk, people who take the action will lower their personal perception of risk [23]. Findings from a recent study supported the behavioral motivation hypothesis at the early stage of COVID-19. Higher levels of perceived risk of infecting coronavirus seem to be associated with greater propensity of respondents to engage in preventive behaviors such as hand washing and social distancing [24]. In our study, the findings supported the reappraisal hypothesis. More preventive behaviors (b = − 3.08, p < .001) and less going out for activities (b = 3.30, p < .001) were associated with lower levels of risk perception.

Taken together, our study found that cognitive and affective variables both played important roles in predicting behavioral responses towards COVID-19. More importantly, affective variables demonstrated stronger explanatory power in predicting both going out and preventive behaviors in our case. Based upon cognitive appraisal theory, there are two stages for people to evaluate how a particular event will affect their well-being: the primary appraisal refers to a person’s evaluation of whether he or she is at risk when facing the event; the secondary appraisal refers to a person’s evaluation of what can be done to cope with the situation or to prevent potential harm [25]. Previous studies considered risk perception as the primary appraisal while people’s knowledge as the secondary appraisal. At the early stage of COVID-19, the primary appraisal was found to be more important in predicting preventive behavior [26]. Findings in our study show that even after the rising phase of the virus outbreak, the first appraisal still played a more dominant role. Another explanation could be that the strict control measures implemented by Chinese government, as well as the effective preventive behaviors people adopted decreased Chinese people’s perceived risk of the incident, which might influence their behavioral responses.

Implications

Findings of this study provide some practical implications for fighting COVID-19. First, since affect seems to be a crucial determinant of preventive behavior adoption, it is important to decrease people’s negative emotion and risk perception. Therefore, the media should be careful when covering the pandemic. Constant reporting of rising numbers of victims, dissatisfaction toward government policies, tear-jerking stories, and finger-pointing sound bites could all possibly drive up high negative emotion among the public. Delivering timely and factual information regarding virus prevention and cautioning against the overflow of negative emotion seem to be important strategies for news coverage during the COVID-19 pandemic.

Second, knowledge is also an important predictor of adopting appropriate behaviors, it is therefore important to improve people’s coronavirus knowledge through public health information campaigns. Such information should target people who are less likely to adopt preventive measures.

Limitations and conclusions

Like other empirical studies, our study has several limitations. First, we cannot ascertain the causal relationships between cognition, affect, and behavior using cross-sectional data. Second, although we have operationalized our variables carefully, we admit that the measures for emotion and knowledge were not perfect. For instance, worry, one of the important negative emotions when facing an epidemic, was not assessed in our study. Third, our study merely focused on internet users in China, and extra caution is needed when generalizing these findings to the whole Chinese population. For people who have no access to the internet, their behavioral pattern could be different because of their lower socioeconomic status and lack of access to information. Third, our study was conducted in China, a society with a set of special cultural and political features. Despite the fact that our findings cannot be blindly generalized to the other societies, we provide empirical evidence of behavioral responses towards COVID-19 in China, one of the most important cases in the world, amidst the current public health crisis.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

COVID-19:

coronavirus disease

WHO:

World Health Organization

CNNIC:

China Internet Network Information Center

References

  1. Baud D, Qi X, Nielsen-Saines K, Musso D, Pomar L, Favre G. Real estimates of mortality following COVID-19 infection. Lancet Infect Dis. 2020;20(7):773. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1473-3099(20)30195-X.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Prem K, Liu Y, Russell TW, Kucharski AJ, Eggo RM, Davies N, et al. The effect of control strategies to reduce social mixing on outcomes of the COVID-19 epidemic in Wuhan, China: a modelling study. Lancet Public Health. 2020;5(5):e261–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30073-6.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Wong LP, Hung C-C, Alias H, Lee TS-H. Anxiety symptoms and preventive measures during the COVID-19 outbreak in Taiwan. BMC Psychiatry. 2020;20(1):376. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-020-02786-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Islam MS, Rahman KM, Sun Y, Qureshi MO, Abdi I, Chughtai AA, et al. Current knowledge of COVID-19 and infection prevention and control strategies in healthcare settings: a global analysis. Infect Control Hosp Epidemiol. 2020;41(10):1196–206. https://doi.org/10.1017/ice.2020.237.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Clements JM. Knowledge and behaviors toward COVID-19 among US residents during the early days of the pandemic: cross-sectional online questionnaire. JMIR Public Health Surveill. 2020;6(2):e19161. https://doi.org/10.2196/19161.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Plohl N, Musil B. Modeling compliance with COVID-19 prevention guidelines: the critical role of trust in science. Psychol Health Med. 2020;1:1–12.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Marinthe G, Brown G, Delouvée S, Jolley D. Looking out for myself: exploring the relationship between conspiracy mentality, perceived personal risk, and COVID-19 prevention measures. Br J Health Psychol. 2020;25(4):957–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12449.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Knowledge, attitude, and practice regarding COVID-19 outbreak in Bangladesh: An online-based cross-sectional study [Internet]. [cited 2021 Feb 23]. Available from: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/article?id=10.1371/journal.pone.0239254

  9. Mukhtar S. Mental health and emotional impact of COVID-19: applying health belief model for medical staff to general public of Pakistan. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;87:28–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Zhang M, Zhou M, Tang F, Wang Y, Nie H, Zhang L, et al. Knowledge, attitude, and practice regarding COVID-19 among healthcare workers in Henan, China. J Hosp Infect. 2020;105(2):183–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhin.2020.04.012.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Dai B, Fu D, Meng G, Liu B, Li Q, Liu X. The effects of governmental and individual predictors on COVID-19 protective behaviors in China: a path analysis model. Public Adm Rev. 2020;80(5):797–804. https://doi.org/10.1111/puar.13236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Qian M, Wu Q, Wu P, Hou Z, Liang Y, Cowling BJ, et al. Psychological responses, behavioral changes and public perceptions during the early phase of the COVID-19 outbreak in China: a population based cross-sectional survey [Internet]. Infectious Diseases (except HIV/AIDS); 2020 Feb [cited 2020 Dec 19]. Available from: http://medrxiv.org/lookup/doi/10.1101/2020.02.18.20024448.

  13. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, McIntyre RS, et al. A longitudinal study on the mental health of general population during the COVID-19 epidemic in China. Brain Behav Immun. 2020;87:40–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbi.2020.04.028.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Li J-B, Yang A, Dou K, Wang L-X, Zhang M-C, Lin X-Q. Chinese public’s knowledge, perceived severity, and perceived controllability of COVID-19 and their associations with emotional and behavioural reactions, social participation, and precautionary behaviour: a national survey. BMC Public Health. 2020;20(1):1589. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-09695-1.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, Ho CS, et al. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(5):1729. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17051729.

    Article  CAS  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Zhou M, Long P, Kong N, Campy KS. Characterizing Wuhan residents’ mask-wearing intention at early stages of the COVID-19 pandemic. Patient Educ Couns [Internet]. 2020 Dec 25 [cited 2021 Feb 23]; Available from: https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0738399120306844

  17. CNNIC. Statistical Report on Internet Development in China (The 44th Survey Report). [cited 2020 Dec 19]. Available from: https://cnnic.com.cn/IDR/ReportDownloads/201911/P020191112539794960687.pdf

  18. Shi Y, Wang J, Yang Y, Wang Z, Wang G, Hashimoto K, et al. Knowledge and attitudes of medical staff in Chinese psychiatric hospitals regarding COVID-19. Brain Behav Immun - Health. 2020;4:100064. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbih.2020.100064.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Brug J, Aro AR, Oenema A, de Zwart O, Richardus JH, Bishop GD. SARS risk perception, knowledge, precautions, and information sources, the Netherlands. Emerg Infect Dis. 2004;10(8):1486–9. https://doi.org/10.3201/eid1008.040283.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Zhong B-L, Luo W, Li H-M, Zhang Q-Q, Liu X-G, Li W-T, et al. Knowledge, attitudes, and practices towards COVID-19 among Chinese residents during the rapid rise period of the COVID-19 outbreak: a quick online cross-sectional survey. Int J Biol Sci. 2020;16(10):1745–52. https://doi.org/10.7150/ijbs.45221.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Duc Huynh TL. “The more I fear about COVID-19, the more I wear medical masks”: A survey on risk perception and medical masks’ uses [Internet]. Health Economics; 2020 Mar [cited 2020 Dec 19]. Available from: http://medrxiv.org/lookup/doi/10.1101/2020.03.26.20044388.

  22. Marks G, Bingman CR, Duval TS. Negative affect and unsafe sex in HIV-positive men. AIDS Behav. 1998;2(2):89–99. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022138712044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Brewer NT, Weinstein ND, Cuite CL, Herrington JE. Risk perceptions and their relation to risk behavior. Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(2):125–30. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15324796abm2702_7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Kwok K, Li K-K, Hin H, Yi Y, Tang A, Wei V, et al. Community responses during early phase of COVID-19 epidemic, Hong Kong. Emerg Infect Dis. 2020;26:2020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Folkman S, Lazarus RS, Dunkel-Schetter C, DeLongis A, Gruen RJ. Dynamics of a stressful encounter: cognitive appraisal, coping, and encounter outcomes. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1986;50(5):992–1003. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.5.992.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Galea S, Merchant RM, Lurie N. The mental health consequences of COVID-19 and physical distancing: the need for prevention and early intervention. JAMA Intern Med. 2020;180(6):817–8. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamainternmed.2020.1562.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This study was supported by the Science Popularization and Risk Communication of Transgenic Biotechnologies project (supervised by Ye Lu, Grant ID: 2016ZX08015002–005), the National Science and Technology Major Project of the Ministry of Science and Technology of China.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

FS conceptualized, analyzed and interpreted the data. He also revised and edited the manuscript. CM was the major contributor in writing the manuscript. YL conceptualized the data. YJ C collected the data. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Chen Min.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Fudan University.

All the methods in the study were conducted according to the criteria set by the ethics committee of Fudan University.

Each subject signed an informed consent before participating to the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shen, F., Min, C., Lu, Y. et al. The effect of cognition and affect on preventive behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic: a cross-sectional study in China. BMC Public Health 21, 722 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10784-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-021-10784-y

Keywords